Abstract

Neurodivergent people often feel they don't belong. This is not just an emotional response to neurotypical norms, but a matter of "misfit" embodiment in particular places (Garland-Thomson, 2011). Recent empirical research adopting a neurodiversity critique is beginning to examine neurodivergent spatialities in increasingly substantive ways. Often space is a static container for the discourses and practices that shape neurodivergent people's lives. Unmarked whiteness often accompanies this approach to space. However, space becomes a much more active presence when examining the experiences of racialized neurodivergent people. Strategies of spatial containment have long been central to regulating racialized and other oppressed groups. However, "container-space" on its own is inadequate for understanding the different risks and outcomes that different neurodivergent individuals face, especially when exhibiting neurodivergent comportment and emotions. Other work builds on these critical insights by seeking to affirm neurodivergent ways of relating to the world. Here, often borrowing from ideas of queer and crip time, scholars show how space can shape how people are continually becoming as embodied subjects over time. This scholarship invites us to ask how specifically neurodivergent forms of agency might emerge from the mutual relationship between embodiment, time and space. Deeper engagement with material space through intersectional thinking and more robust attention to space when exploring alternative temporalities are needed not only to understand how neurodivergent people can be misfits, but also how they may thrive.


Introduction

As we are embodied beings, our neurotypes necessarily take place, and the spatial relations that constitute human neurodiversity are many and varied. It's a commonplace that neurodivergent people often feel they don't belong in neurotypical space. They're a square peg in a round hole—a "misfit" as Rosemarie Garland-Thomson would put it (Garland-Thomson, 2011, p. 593). Neurodivergent people often feel separate or alone, or at least are told that they do. Jim Sinclair's 2010 commentary in this journal, "Being Autistic Together," by proposing what was (and still is) widely regarded as impossible, demonstrates how neurotypical ontologies of autism are inherently spatialized in this way. Xe lists "autistic 'aloneness,' 'withdrawal,' and 'disconnectedness,' autistic people 'living in their own worlds,' being 'trapped' inside 'shells' or behind 'invisible walls'" as examples of the neurotypical world's assumption that "autistic people are unable to be 'together' with other people" (Sinclair, 2010). These are spatial ontologies because this isolation—understood as such in neurotypical terms—is what defines the being of autistic people, what makes autistics who they are. Sinclair goes on to explore spaces in which autistic people do indeed get together with other autistics, whether in "places for autistics" designed by well-meaning neurotypical people or in self-created "autistic spaces"—both of which xe is careful to point out present their own challenges for autistics. How space is framed, thought and represented—as well as materially shaped—has very real consequences for the opportunities and constraints afforded by neurotypical society.

The growing body of critical work on neurodivergence and neuronormativity deploys spatiality in different ways. Incorporating intersectional concerns such as gendering, queering and racialization further expands the spatialities through which we understand neurodiversity. Research in real-world spaces claiming to take a neurodiversity perspective has increased markedly in the past few years. Heeding anxieties about neurodiversity becoming a buzzword (Walker & Raymaker, 2021, p. 7), I focus here on work that actively critiques the pathologization of neurodivergent people. I have not aimed for complete ideological purity in vocabulary or even substance, as I recognize that the critical perspectives and politics that neurodiversity entails still need to be "sold" in language the academy understands. This is particularly so for researchers in the psy disciplines seeking to shift away from the pathologizing gaze of the medical paradigm. But this is also so in other disciplines, and the result is a non-systematic but non-arbitrary multidisciplinary selection of largely social science work that in one way or another addresses material spatialities of neurodiversity, rather than simply representational ones, important as they are. Furthermore, I do not wish to dismiss the distress that particular impairments can bring (Graby, 2015), not least due to my own personal experience with being neurodivergent. I know all too well that medicalizing language can have its uses when describing impairments.

Unsurprisingly this work is mostly about autism. The neurodiversity movement arose in the autistic community, and much of the funding for research is directed toward autism. Thus ADHD, dyslexia and other diagnoses/(dis)identifications make sporadic appearances. As I explore the ways neurodiversity is spatialized in this body of research, I will draw upon a long-standing geographical metaphor as I trace how space is treated as a static container for neurodivergence (Malpas, 2012). Even the most relational accounts can still rely upon container-space as its physical anchor. I will also attend to the uneven presence of intersectional analyses. The social science neurodiversity literature is overwhelmingly unmarked by its middle-class whiteness, while the gendering of neurodivergence makes an occasional appearance. I will consider a handful of spatial accounts that explicitly consider the racialization of neurodivergence and the very different experiences and outcomes that Black and Brown autistics, ADHDers and others experience compared to their white counterparts. Container-space is inadequate for understanding global majority experiences of neurodivergence and public and state perceptions and treatment of them. Spatiality is also intimately bound to temporality, not least because neurodiversity is often framed as a threat to white, neurotypical futurity. But the main focus here is on work that conceives of neurodivergent subjectivities as always being in a process of becoming in response to the affordances of the rest of the material world. A fuller understanding of this mutual constitution of space and subjectivity—how neurodivergent people and space produce and change each other—requires more attention to material spatiality and intersectionality in neurodiversity research.

Neurodiversity and Spatiality

Neurodiversity simply means the variations in cognition and perception present in the human population. It is both a movement and a mode of critique that takes its cue in part from the social model of disability (Chapman, 2020; Oliver, 1990) to depathologize neurological traits such as autism, ADHD, dyslexia, dyspraxia, dyscalculia, Tourette's and others, and to challenge the social norms that are disabling for neurodivergent people. The history of the movement has been covered elsewhere (Botha et al., 2024; S. Kapp, 2020; Walker, 2021), as have various conceptual and political critiques (Russell, 2020). What I want to do here is to explore the spatialities characteristic of empirical work that seeks to use the neurodiversity paradigm as a major mode of analysis and critique.

By "spatiality," I mean what the geographer Rob Kitchin (2020, p. 321) describes as "The mutual constitution of space and social relations, recognizing the codeterminate ways in which space is shaped." Mahmood Shoorcheh notes that spatiality is at once "abstract and concrete, produced and producing, imagined and materialized, structured and lived, relational, relative and absolute" (Shoorcheh, 2019, p. 64). Spatiality, in Henri Lefebvre's (1991) words, is space that is perceived, conceived and lived. My interest in neurodiversity's spatialities comes from my disciplinary location as a geographer. Geographical work on neurodiversity is relatively scant and punctuated over time. Autistic sensory geographies (Birkett, et al., 2022; J. Davidson & Henderson, 2010b), more-than-human geographies (J. Davidson & Smith, 2009; Judge, 2018) and geographies of the ambivalent relationship some autistics have to gender (J. Davidson & Tamas, 2016) illustrate ways neurodivergence troubles normative socio-spatial relations in productive ways. Studies of autobiographies of bipolar (Chouinard, 2012) and autism (J. Davidson, 2007) have explored the spatial imaginaries in first-hand accounts, while accounts of coming out experiences (J. Davidson & Henderson, 2010a) and self-disclosure in the discipline (England, 2016) have focused on taking on a neurodivergent identity in the public sphere. Focusing on neurodivergent individuals' agency has underscored the relational production of urban space as a process of becoming supportive or oppressive (Kenna, 2022). Finally, studies of online community building and the political possibilities of cyberspace (J. Davidson, 2008; Henderson et al., 2014) have looked at virtual geographies of neurodiversity.

The latter studies anticipated a significant shift in recent work embracing the neurodiversity approach. While Nick Walker's and other neurodiversity advocates' work on activism and community-formation originally focused largely on material spaces (S. Kapp, 2020), virtual space appears to be coming to the fore in neurodiversity research, a process perhaps accelerated by the COVID-19 pandemic. This work, while important, will not be addressed here. Online platforms are crucial for connecting people across space, particularly for those whose sensory and social needs are unmet in neurotypical space. But as Anne McGuire (2016, p. 63) reminds us of the material geography of online communities, "The balance of power in these communities [the neurodiversity movement] is thus invariably mapped along class, race, and gender lines." But in much of this work, that material spatiality, when mentioned at all, at best serves as a touchstone for an otherwise representational analysis. Similarly, scholars reframing epistemologies of neurodiversity aim to create discursive and institutional spaces for neurodivergent thriving (Bertilsdottir Rosqvist et al., 2023; Lawson Jacobs, 2023; Manase, 2024), in which space is largely deployed as a metaphor for the web of power relations they seek to transform. The spatialities of virtual, representational, and epistemological neurodiversity require their own papers.

Empirical Work on Material Spaces

It should come as no surprise that educational spaces and workplaces dominate in social science empirical work on neurodiversity. What perhaps is surprising is that therapeutic spaces such as the psychiatrist's office are almost completely absent from my selection of literature, which may be due to the disciplinary prominence of education in the neurodiversity literature. However, a growing body of historical and genealogical work is showing how neurodivergence has been identified as such in the context of capitalist economies' needs for self-regulating and productive subjects (Grinker, 2020; Jack, 2014; Waltz, 2013). The school has become the primary biopolitical space of therapeutic intervention and regulation to try to make neurodivergent children fit for future participation in the labor market—whether or not the intervention is supportive of neurodivergent children as neurodivergent (Douglas, 2010; Jaysane-Darr, 2020; McGuire, 2016). In some workplaces employers are increasingly refiguring autism—and to a lesser extent, ADHD—as signs for particular capacities to produce surplus value. This has led them to take a second look at neurodivergent people who have long been excluded from the labor market (Grant & Kara, 2021). I do not dismiss the importance of hard-won disability rights in either context, but I want to emphasize a key question that a neurodiversity perspective and other critical approaches prompt us to ask. In what do we seek to be included?

Educational spaces

The question of inclusive spatiality is one of bodies and spaces as much as it is one of policies and procedures. There is more empirical research on schools focusing on materiality from a neurodiversity perspective than there is research on the workplace (cf. Benozzo et al., 2021; Bertilsdotter Rosqvist, 2019; Grant & Kara, 2021; Lambert & Harriss, 2022; Seers & Hogg, 2021). Engaging with wider debates on inclusive schooling is beyond the scope of this paper. What I want to outline here is how schools themselves are framed as material spaces that help or hinder neurodivergent learning and wellbeing, and how spaces of learning in turn are not limited to schools. Much of this scholarship seeks to validate neurodiversity in the classroom, offer evidence for ways to make the classroom a more accessible environment, and affirm the worth of neurodivergent students qua neurodivergent and center their voices. But as several scholars have shown, this affirmation is not universally available. Some may leverage both medical diagnoses and neurodivergent identities to students' advantage. However, neurodiversity-affirming labels are only available to some, while pathologizing diagnoses can have severe consequences for those excluded from what can only be called the category of the "deserving neurodivergent."

Centering the voices of autistic students was the priority of one research team. Costley et al. (2023) sought to do so by positioning their participants as "co-researchers" who helped shape the research agenda around experiences of anxiety in school. In defining the problem, students reported a familiar range of social, sensory and pedagogical challenges, while their spatial framing of the school was that of a container for a difficult social environment exacerbated by specific physical characteristics. The team took an "ecological systems approach" to participatory research, which situates the autistic co-researcher in their family and community contexts that are both fields of intervention (Costley et al., 2023, p. 694). But space largely takes the guise of bodily mobility within the school, especially when seeking "safe places" (Costley et al., 2023, p. 698).

In a separate results paper, Costley et al. (2021, p. 2) note that in the previous literature, school support for autistic students generally comes in two forms, minimizing adverse environmental factors, and encouraging resilience. These are explicitly spatial in that they typically involve permission to minimize physical exposure to stressors of the sort initially reported by the students. But in terms of the team's thematic analysis of their data, the students reported uncertainty, assessment, and others' behaviors (including bullying) as causes of anxieties. None of these derived themes are specifically spatial. Rather, the analysis relegates space to the details of some of the support on offer, such as calm and ordered classrooms, permission to pass between classes earlier than other students, and time-out passes to physically withdraw from the classroom, as well as self-help such as "fidgeting" (stimming) out of sight such as under a table (Costley et al., 2021, pp. 7–8). Again, the school is largely a container for anxiety-provoking relationships and expectations.

An entirely separate methodological paper on participatory research with autistic students is unsurprising, given that most research on schools still center parents and teachers. Jan Wilson (2017) engages with Universal Design for Learning (UDL), a difference-affirming approach to education first proposed in the 1990s, as a way forward for including neurodivergent students in the classroom. She starts with a familiar story of a multiply neurodivergent child repeatedly escaping the classroom in an under-resourced school not designed for her needs. The child is hers, and her daughter's "inclusion" in the regular classroom was anything but. She writes, "This vantage point has led me to call here for a 'radical' shift in focus away from pathologizing discourses and policies that attempt to fit students with disabilities into classrooms and curricula designed for a mythical 'able-bodied,' neurotypical, white, male, middle-class 'norm' and toward a consideration of how classrooms, pedagogy, curricular materials, and cultural practices of schools can be transformed in order to accommodate all students" (Wilson, 2017).

Wilson outlines three multiplicities required for UDL: multiple methods of representation (different ways of communicating information), multiple means of engagement (motivating students) and multiple means of expression (students communicating their knowledge). These involve material practices ranging from showing visual media, to encouraging students to shape teaching, to allowing students to show their knowledge through performance. All these materialities shape the learning environment in profound ways. Likewise, Tomlinson and Newman (2017) advocate UDL on similar grounds (see also von Below et al., 2021). They recruited adult writers to talk about their needs at university, work and elsewhere. The participants highlighted qualities of the physical environment, ranging from appropriate office supplies to the ability to move around the space. Sound was another priority, but not necessarily as something to avoid: "Four discussed auditory stimuli they found helpful—two listened to instrumental music, while two others either composed aloud or recorded themselves while writing" (Tomlinson & Newman, 2017, pp. 100–101).

While this underscores UDL's embodied and material aspects, the focus is largely on process that takes place in the container of the place of learning. UDL advocates are clear that the container overflows; learning takes place outside the classroom, while contextual factors affect what happens inside (for a related if somewhat more individualizing strengths-based approach, see Sewell & Park, 2021). Indeed, some studies examine neurodivergent learning outside the classroom, such as a summer camp in the US (Simpson et al., 2024), and in the bush in the case of an indigenous Australian autistic boy: "Willow [his mother] was enthusiastic about 'going on Country' as a form of therapy for her son, telling traditional stories and imparting knowledge about local flora and fauna as a way to connect him with his heritage and to help him 'self-regulate'" (Lilley et al., 2020, p. 1865).

All these learning spaces are designed (or at least selected) to contain objects, practices and relationships that facilitate neurodivergent learning. There is also closer attention to ways educators relabel their students' behaviors. Sewell and Park (2021) examine how an educational psychologist trained staff to see "challenging" ADHD behavior in a different way. Teachers began to understand such "symptoms" as stress responses to the students' environment, "in which repeated failure and censure from teachers had an erosive effect on motivation and engagement" (Sewell & Park, 2021, p. 686). Likewise Pluquailec (2018) examines the potential for restorying neurodivergent behaviors and emotions. She takes inspiration from Sara Ahmed's (2004) concept of an affective economy to approach the classroom as a relational space. Here, a child's bite might be taken to express an emotion as an in-the-moment socio-spatial mediation between her, her teacher and the classroom environment. This reframes the bite from a sign of an inherent pathology of "challenging behavior" that has discursively sedimented over time in the school. The spatiality here is one of circulating affects and signs, but still contained within the space of the school.

The term "inclusion" evokes space-as-container, which is not a criticism. Containing bodies within, partitioning bodies between, and channelling bodies through particular spaces are key ways of regulating society. Furthermore, what bodies are in a space can define that space, while what space bodies are in can define those bodies, hence the sense of being out of place when the two do not align. This is where the limitation lies. Making the school, university or workplace a safe container for neurodiversity as a generic category of difference by making it a less "toxic" environment (Shiels et al., 2021) is a laudable goal. However, reifying neurodiversity as an object of containment only goes so far. If we reframe this more precisely as seeking a space that can contain all forms of neurodivergent embodiment—with their range of cognitive styles, sensory issues, emotional expressions, and needs for movement—the problems become clearer. All these are simultaneously produced through and embedded in other power relations.

The consequences are two-fold. First, this suggests that principles such as "universal design" are more utopian visions than achievable goals. Wilson (2017, p. 8) is clear that UDL is meant to be all-inclusive, not just of disability, but there is a sense in which the "discredited bodies" she seeks to include are disabled or racialized or gendered. The classroom becomes a container for discrete differences. Second, and perhaps more importantly, the freedoms of emotional expression and mobility scholars advocate for neurodivergent people are not equally accessible (Grant & Kara, 2021). Spatializing the school or the workplace as a container not only provides a discrete refuge for different embodiments. It discursively separates those "neurodiverse" bodies from the other power relations that materially position them in particular ways, with different vulnerabilities. Lilley et al.'s (2020) indigenous Australian participant alludes to this separation of identities when she points toward Country, away from the formal classroom, as space where her autistic son can be his whole self. Other intersectional accounts of neurodiversity offer ways of thinking beyond container-space.

Beyond container-space

The spatiality of Kimberlé Crenshaw's (1991) intersection metaphor has been addressed repeatedly over the years (see Nash, 2017), and neurodiversity has been added to the mix of power relations (Erevelles & Minear, 2010; Krazinski, 2023; Udonsi, 2022). Here I focus on the limits of space-as-container in neurodiversity work, and how moving beyond this spatiality goes hand-in-hand with some modes of intersectional thinking. When considering neurodivergence as simultaneously racialized, classed and subject to cis-heteronormativity, its spatiality can be difficult to contain. A crucial insight of intersectional neurodiversity work is that a container for difference is not an innocent space. Creating a distinct space that ostensibly welcomes neurodiversity is not necessarily an innocent act, especially when it contains and thereby segregates difference. Attending to intersecting differences within such a space undermines the fantasy of container-space's discreteness and boundedness.

Several scholars have researched racialization as a key intersecting process. Leon Hilton examines the case of a Black autistic non-speaking teenager, Avonte Oquendo, who had a need to move, to run. After going missing for several months after escaping from his New York City school, his body was found, and the medical examiner determined he most likely drowned in the East River, perhaps due to his fascination with water infrastructure, along with trains and cars (Hilton, 2017, p. 222). A federal law was proposed to fund "voluntary" electronic tracking devices for autistic students and alarm systems for special education classrooms. However, Hilton argues, "Avonte's Law directly, if tacitly, emanates from principles of surveillance and extends the police as a modern institution and power formation that has historically constellated itself around the surveillance of blackness, and especially of the black body in motion" (Hilton, 2017, p. 224). A blurred surveillance camera image of Avonte running from school saturated the city, "index[ing] an unruly and uncontrollable subject, incapable of controlling an impulse toward movement" (Hilton, 2017, pp. 225–226).

Here autism's inscrutability combines with the Black body's hypervisibility, reflecting a long, intertwined history of racist and ableist forms of surveillance and spectacle that denied Avonte his personhood. Autistic wandering inherits the state surveillance attracted by what Fred Moten calls the Black "fugitive law of movement" (Moten, 2009, p. 230, cited in Hilton, 2017) that seeks to contain unruly bodies. Hilton does not dismiss the dilemmas around safety posed by wandering (Wilson, 2017), but seeks to problematize converging white and neurotypical urges toward containment and denial of autonomy. The task is to find alternative ways of accommodating the need to move on neurodivergent people's own terms, without relying upon racializing practices and technologies of control (see also (Erevelles & Minear, 2010; Solomon & Lawlor, 2018).

Neurodivergent embodiment exceeds the containers that white, neurotypical society seeks to confine it in. However, power relations that expose neurodivergent individuals to risks of harm in different ways also work to undermine bounded conceptions of the spaces in which they are supposedly contained. Ralph Savarese writes of his autistic son benefitting in school from having highly educated and well-to-do parents by dint of sheer luck (Savarese, 2018, p. 28). Class and racial privilege enable them to enter into the educational system, navigate it and change it (however piecemeal) from within. But however supportive a school may be, it is pursuing a regulatory project.

This project includes white students. Jaysane-Darr (2020, p. 272) shows how therapists working in wealthy white communities understand neurodivergent emotions as both threat to and opportunity for whiteness. For them, "emotional diversity is either an asset or a barrier that must be managed in the pursuit of higher education and economic success." Their professional practice contributes to a "biopolitics of emotion," in which assessing and disciplining students' emotional expressions and even experiences are key. This is so, regardless of whether the therapist's object of intervention is "emotionally suspect behaviour or neurodiversity" (Jaysane-Darr, 2020, p. 272). This is a rare look at interpersonal encounters between therapist and client from a neurodiversity perspective. Jaysane-Darr's participants worked in mostly affluent white areas, and their approach to their (presumably white) clients were highly individualizing. While they were required to contain clients at the metaphorical level by categorizing them according to tick boxes on forms, they were also "navigating the spectrum" and "navigating emotional diversity" by gaining an understanding of neurodivergent people's embodied, sensory experiences of space.

However, some parents can use the discourse of neurodiversity to advantage when their child is perceived to be disturbing the space of the classroom. Elsa Davidson (2021, p. 1141) characterizes parental wielding of "sensory issues" as an "auratically scientific force that neutralizes potentially negative moral judgments about children, and by extension, parents—for those in a position to deploy this force." But this claim to innocence operates at a larger scale than the classroom. Davidson (2021, p. 1141) shows how the mismatch of the classroom's sensory environment to neurodivergent students' sensoria has a larger geography in white and middle-class neighborhoods and schools. She argues the deployment of children's sensory issues for their advantage is racialized and classed. "Such benefits point to the implication of the child sensorium in a broader 'biopolitics of disability' (Mitchell & Snyder, 2015 cited in Puar, 2017, p. xvii) in which those with class and racial privilege have their disability/difference recognized and can pursue a label of neurodiversity as a form of risk management, a privilege that must be understood as generally unavailable to poor children and children of color who experience slow debilitation through daily exposure to exclusionary and under-resourced schools, lack of adequate shelter, and inadequate forms of support under conditions of neoliberal disinvestment, and whose disabilities may be responded to with state violence instead of recognition (Puar, 2017)" (E. Davidson, 2021, p. 1141; see also Udonsi, 2022; Waltz, 2020, p. 24).

Others have made the point that neurodivergent diagnoses can be dangerous for global majority students. However, many of these critiques of the racialization of neuronormativity point toward sanism, especially anti-Black sanism (Pickens, 2019). Meerai et al. (2016) explore this in terms of mental health diagnoses such as schizophrenia in schools in Toronto, where Black boys are four times as likely to be diagnosed as other students. Here diagnosis connects the space of the school to the carceral spaces of the psychiatric hospital; Black boys and men are far more likely to be confined against their will and such "psychiatric aggressions toward Black bodies" are crucial to the school-to-prison pipeline (Meerai et al., 2016, p. 19). The authors situate their argument in terms of mad studies. However, my point here is that racialized neurodivergent embodiment puts pressure on container space in distinctive ways by showing how even carceral space—the ultimate strategy for containing unruly and threatening bodies—is never simply a discrete space in which they are contained in isolation from wider power relations, (geo)politics and histories.

Becoming in Space and Time

The work explored so far makes a key point with varying degrees of explicitness. Neurodivergent spatiality is intimately bound up with time. Neurodivergence isn't static; is a production, both individual and biopolitical. Neurodivergence becomes in and through space. A case in point is Gemma North's (2023) research participants, autistic women whom she interviewed about reasonable adjustments in the workplace. North offers an intersectional reading of multiple power relations that exceed the boundaries of the workplace. Sexist expectations that began in childhood followed them into the workplace, with their autistic embodiment defying the gendered expectations of co-workers. The brusqueness of one participant, which would have been accepted from a man, was considered "rude" when coming from a woman (North, 2023, p. 950). Particularly irritating to participants was when employers assumed they knew what adjustments were needed, including giving an employee a separate office when they didn't want to be isolated from their co-workers. Some were treated as taking up too much space, because their autistic embodiment defied employers' gendered expectations of women's comportment. Socio-spatial relations changed along with the women's sense of self. Some discovered that once they educated their colleagues about their differences the emotional labor of masking was no longer necessary, making for a healthier workplace and sense of agency. However, some "got stuck" in time, as career progression stalled thanks to ableist interview and promotion practices. Little of this should come as a surprise. These women and their workplaces were not static, and when they were static, this violated normative expectations of growth and change.

Such becoming, thwarted or not, can also be moment-to-moment. As has been noted above, neurodivergent movements and emotions are non-normative ways of negotiating the relationship between the body and the rest of the environment—including what is in the past and what is yet to come. Stimming is a classic example, as autistics seek to maintain a feeling bodily equilibrium; while this is something everyone does, it becomes pathology when autistics do it differently. North's interviewees were clear that they had to hide or quell their stimming at work (North, 2023, p. 952). However, some workplaces allow for stimming—their affordances, as Miranda Brady (2022) suggests, accommodate neurodivergence. Brady's interviewees were stage performers who found that so long as they had a quiet green room to be alone in before a performance, or in some cases the freedom to pace backstage, performing on a stage before a live audience produced an opportunity for social connection and creativity.

The productive capacity of environmental affordances troubles the individualizing narrative of the medical model of disability. By embracing this insight in terms of her ADHD, Michelle Attias (2020) has come to identify as neuroqueer. This has prompted "a process of understanding a vast assemblage of experiences and encounters in which I found myself and continue to find myself to be a misfit" (Attias, 2020, p. 57). But Attias's focus on the method of 'mind-meandering' is primarily about time, specifically "the need for crip time…breaking away from structured [neurotypical] time" (Attias, 2020, p. 65). For Attias, "Time is elliptical, with the past experienced in all of its emotional intensity as a living future" (Attias, 2020, p. 66). This intense temporality is what the neurotypical world pathologizes as distraction, but Attias refigures as a valuable way of being in the world. In one meander, Attias writes that "embodiment, time and space and sensory experience fold in on one another, and even though I attempt to delineate between them, their boundaries erode" (Attias, 2020, p. 67). Further on Attias exhorts us to: "Embrace your mutability. Get lost in all that you don't know. Wade through the swamp without direction. Know that there is no right direction. Let go of geographies, maps, instructions, plans, answers" (Attias, 2020, p. 67). The result here of losing distinctions between time, space and the mind is that mind becomes space and space becomes metaphor.

A key point of mind-meandering is to recover agency and intentionality for ADHDers and other neurodivergent people on their own terms and resist normate measures of subjectivity. Attias's argument enables this in part by shying away from space qua space in favor of time. Here agency and intentionality refer to the capacity to change something across time. They also refer to engaging with timeframes beyond the present/future dyad that dominates so much of neuronormative capitalist framings of productivity. Memory and artefacts are key means for reaching across time; through them, we have the capacity to "bend time" (Attias, 2020, p. 62; see also Kafer, 2013, p. 27; McRuer & Johnson, 2014 on cripistemology). But fully embracing space in the process of queering the normate world poses more challenging questions for our ideas of agency. If material space itself constitutes us, our mind, our subjectivity, where is our agency and intentionality, our subjectivity? If it's not contained in our mind, can we truly say it's ours?

Constitutive spaces

Ben Belek takes a more Latourian approach to neurodivergence to explore how autistic adults often focus more on sensory than social differences in defining themselves. Latour's (2007) actor-network theory dethrones "the human" as the sole location of agency. It locates agency in the non-human as well as the human, both of which are themselves "assemblages" or networks, and both of which can be a part of other networks. Change emerges from shifting relations within these networks. Networks/assemblages are a form of spatiality, and so Latour's account makes change a question of space as much as one of time. In this ontology, a body cannot be a discrete, contained object. Belek glosses Latour's (2004) definition of the body, which may or may not be human: "the quintessential property of the body, which singles it out from other objects, is its continual process of learning to be affected" (Belek, 2019, p. 30). 1 This has spatial consequences. "As one learns to discriminate between increasingly subtle contrasts in the surrounding, the environment itself is modified" (Belek, 2019, pp. 30–31). Particular aspects of one's environment become more differentiated and perceptible: "A more sensitive body, therefore, ultimately means a richer world" (Belek, 2019, p. 31).

Belek's focus is on adult autistics' "process of learning themselves as autistic" in support groups (Belek, 2019, p. 37). Participants learned from each other to articulate what they previously thought of as individual idiosyncrasies in their sensorium as specifically autistic experiences. They reconfigured their bodily relationship to their environment by learning a new vocabulary for bodily distress, ranging from "trigger" to "meltdown." Doing so in an autistic social environment was key to increasing their sensitivity to their surroundings. As their perception of their environment was reshaped, so too was their embodied agency to react to it and reshape it in turn. "Autistic communities…enable the bringing about of autistic bodies, where before, there were only bodies with autism" (Belek, 2019, p. 31).

Erin Felepchuk (2021) also takes a more-than-human approach to stimming by arguing that this negotiation between a person's bodymind and their environment is an improvisation. Framing stimming as improvisation counters the neuronormative construction of stimming as "mere" repetition, an understanding that denies autistics agency, but also severs the specific relationship that a bodymind has to its environment in a particular place and time. Crucially, Felepchuk (2021) argues that "stimming is an improvisatory interaction with the affordances of our environment. In other words, we stim according to the opportunities or barriers that are presented to us by our environments." These affordances can change, as in the case of COVID-19. Masks, according to Felepchuk, make using chewelry impossible and can therefore prompt a new form of stimming (e.g. chewing gum) for maintaining sensory equilibrium within the new environment, and thus enabling access to spaces that would otherwise be inaccessible due to mask mandates.

This is not to claim stimming as an inherent good. Felepchuk acknowledges the potential for it to be self-injurious or interpreted by police as a threat when someone from the global majority performs this "improvisation." To revisit a point raised earlier, neurodivergent improvisations can form an assemblage with melanin in relation to the affordances of a white supremacist society, with disastrous results (Saldanha, 2006). As Vanessa Thompson (2021, p. 65) notes, "as various initiatives against racial profiling also emphasise, racist policing extends the actual policing incident in time and space, across individuals and across generations." Belek does not raise such possibilities, and so power relations other than neuronormativity are absent from the spatial production of autistic bodies and worlds in his account. Autistic space, like container space in the previous section, becomes reified as a discrete thing that paradoxically floats free of other power relations.

Remi Yergeau (2020) shares this broad commitment to the affective relationship between body and space as they explore the discursive production of a folk disorder, Cassandra Affective Deprivation Disorder (CADD). They are particularly attentive to how this deeply ableist construction intersects with a profound heteronormativity. Advocates for the existence of this "disorder" figure embodied proximity to autistics as a risk. Neurotypical women bear this risk in heterosexual relationships with autistic men, whose supposed extreme male brains pose a risk to heterosexual futurity. In allegedly depriving their women partners of affection, these men "deprive neurotypicals of intimacy and sanity" (Yergeau, 2020, p. 213). Self-styled sufferers of CADD frame themselves as victims of abuse, however unintentional, whose suffering is not taken seriously by others. This perceived gaslighting compounds their social and spatial isolation, as "relatives cannot bear witness to all of the sex that one is not having, to all of the non-intimacies that might percolate in an autistic-NT partnership" (Yergeau, 2020, p. 213). This silencing produces an arhetoricity that resembles autism itself, as CADD advocates figure autistics as inherently incommunicative and lacking moral accountability (Yergeau, 2018). Autism becomes a contagion for heteronormativity, as "being in the mere presence of autism is enough to descend the non-autistic into madness as well as queerness" (Yergeau, 2020, p. 123). The ableist and anti-queer assumptions underlying this diagnosis construct a peculiar spatiality for autistic-NT intimacy. Such intimacy is something to avoid: "Quick, neurotypicals—swipe left! swipe left!" (Yergeau, 2020, 213). It is also a dispositive (Oswin & Olund, 2010), where intimacy is a productive object of regulation through which autistic (a)sexuality can and should be rehabilitated by those few heroic experts who believe these Cassandras (Yergeau, 2020, p. 219). 2

Finally, Brendan Hart (2014) provocatively argues that some forms of applied behavior analysis (ABA) can paradoxically further a neurodiversity approach to producing autistic personhood. Parents engage in "joint embodiment" with their autistic children to facilitate their presence as autistic in the social world and become part of their child's "prosthetic environment." These include physical objects ranging from "sensory toys, to picture-based daily planners, to adaptive spoons with thick, easy-to-grasp handles" (Hart, 2014, p. 298). As part of this prosthetic environment, such objects can combine with practices in the form of therapies. They, according to Hart (2014, p. 298), "provide an 'artificial platform' and a technical infrastructure for the public establishment of autistic personhood…[in] social worlds not built to their specifications. In the process, these worlds are themselves being reconfigured." It's beyond the scope of this paper to debate ABA. 3 What interests me here is the conceptual claim about the mutual constitution of personhood and space. Everybody inhabits a prosthetic environment, and this is a key thing to acknowledge. Just as some forms of stimming do not "fit" and thus fairly demand a response, a prosthetic environment that can facilitate an autistic's engagement with the world on their own terms will also elicit a response from that environment. As with Belek, there is a sole focus on affirming neurodiversity, but the spatial affordances here as with Felepchuk are open-ended, allowing other power relations to have a place, for good or for ill.

Conclusion

The sensory, cognitive and other bodily differences that neurodivergent people experience do not just happen; they take place. In its literal sense, taking place is neurotypical society's problem with neurodivergence to begin with. Empirical scholarship on autism, ADHD, dyslexia and related categories of difference that take a critical neurodiversity perspective has proliferated, if belatedly, in response to years of increasingly insistent neurodivergent self-advocacy (S. Kapp, 2020). And yet attention to the material, spatial aspects of neurodivergence has been uneven at best. Space is often conceived as a static container for difference. This is unsurprising given that managing the presence and movement of bodies through spatial containment is a primary strategy for governing heterogeneous populations (Foucault, 1977; Povinelli, 2011). Contributions to our knowledge about neurodivergence—people's direct experiences as well as others' responses to it—have been considerable in terms of discourses and practices. However, these largely play out in relation to material spaces that are decidedly static.

This "container-space" (Malpas, 2012) is a passive background for active subjects, particularly ones that are white and middle class yet unmarked as such. Material space can take on a much more assertive presence when intersections of race, gender and other differences become part of the analysis. This active spatiality comes to the fore in relation to power relations and differences between those persons situated inside and others situated outside spaces such as schools and workplaces. Equally importantly, spatiality's dynamism is key for understanding differences between those within a given space. For example, the freedom to engage in neurodivergent movement or emotional expression, when available at all, is likely to be limited to white men in particular spaces. Similar embodied expressions of autism and ADHD can require masking for white women, leading to a greater expenditure of energy, underdiagnosis, and a lack of support. For Black people, such movements can result in over-diagnosis in schools and other spaces. Rather than leading to support, diagnosis can amount to a form of surveillance that polices movement within and between spaces with potentially deadly results. The more comfortable one is in a space because of one's social location and identity, the more static it can seem.

Other work, often seeking an affirmative approach to neurodivergent ways of relating to the world, has shown how space can shape how people are continually becoming as embodied subjects over time. This can be an in-the-moment mediation between body and environment through stimming, or a life-long process of identity formation in different spaces over time. Finally, some work mobilizes various versions of assemblage theory to show how neurodivergent embodiment and space are mutually constitutive. One's presence as a neurodivergent person in public space, for example, by affecting and being affected can change that space over time, possibly to something more affirmative.

The implications of a deeper understanding of the geography of neurodiversity are twofold. First, empirical research from a neurodiversity perspective would do well to pay more attention to material space and how it shapes as it is shaped by power relations beyond neuronormativity. As with neurodiverse identities, neurodiverse spaces simply do not exist in isolation from race, gender, sexuality and other differences. As noted previously, the intimate historical-spatial relationships between intersecting relations of power such as racism and heteronormativity have been explored in a range of disciplines (Chen, 2012; Kline, 2001; Povinelli, 2011). Empirical work on neurodiversity will be at best incomplete without paying heed to this work, as neurodiversity isn't lived in isolation from other power relations, contained in its own space. Second, there is more to say about the relationship between neurodivergent space and time. Critical notions of queer time and crip time (Kafer, 2013) have inspired new ways of looking at resistance to the normalizing forces of capitalism and neuroablism, but a serious reckoning with their materializations in space is just beginning. The ways our neurodivergent becomings take time are inseparable from how they take place.

Acknowledgments

I thank Dyi Huijg, Jeff Brune, and the two anonymous referees for their constructive comments.

References

  • Ahmed, S. (2004). The cultural politics of emotion. Edinburgh University Press.
  • Attias, M. (2020). Mind-meandering as AD(H)D methodology: An embodied, neuroqueer practice of art-making and resistance in dialogue with Kurt Cobain's and Lee Lozano's journals. Research in Arts and Education, 2020(4), 53–85. https://doi.org/10.54916/rae.119283
  • Belek, B. (2019). Articulating sensory sensitivity: From bodies with autism to autistic bodies. Medical Anthropology, 38(1), 30–43. https://doi.org/10.1080/01459740.2018.1460750
  • Benozzo, A., Koro, M., Vasquez, A., Vitrukh, M., Barbetta, P., & Long, C. (2022). A femin… manifesto: Academic ecologies of care and cure during a global health pandemic. Gender, Work, and Organization, 29(4), 1236-1258. https://doi.org/10.1111/gwao.12762
  • Bertilsdotter Rosqvist, H. (2019). Doing things together: Exploring meanings of different forms of sociality among autistic people in an autistic work space. ALTER: European Journal of Disability Research, 13(3), 168–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.alter.2019.03.003
  • Bertilsdotter Rosqvist, H., Chown, N. & Stenning, A. (Eds.). (2020). Neurodiversity studies: A new critical paradigm. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429322297
  • Bertilsdotter Rosqvist, H., Hultman, L., Österborg Wiklund, S., Nygren, A., Storm, P., & Sandberg, G. (2023). Naming ourselves, becoming neurodivergent scholars. Disability & Society, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/09687599.2023.2271155
  • Birkett, L., McGrath, L., & Tucker, I. (2022). Muting, filtering and transforming space: Autistic children's sensory "tactics" for navigating mainstream school space following transition to secondary school. Emotion, Space and Society, 42, 1-8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.emospa.2022.100872
  • Botha, M., Chapman, R., Giwa Onaiwu, M., Kapp, S. K., Stannard Ashley, A., & Walker, N. (2024). The neurodiversity concept was developed collectively: An overdue correction on the origins of neurodiversity theory. Autism, 28(6), 1591-1594. https://doi.org/10.1177/13623613241237871
  • Brady, M. J. (2022). Onstage and behind the scenes: Autistic performance and advocacy. Studies in Social Justice, 16(2), 429–446. https://doi.org/10.26522/ssj.v16i2.2666
  • Chapman, R. (2020). Neurodiversity, disability, wellbeing. In H. Bertilsdotter Rosqvist, N. Chown, & A. Stenning (Eds.), Neurodiversity studies: A new critical paradigm (pp. 57–72). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429322297-7
  • Chapman, R. & Bovell, V. (2022). Neurodiversity, advocacy, anti-therapy. In J. Matson & P. Sturmey (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorder (pp. 1519-1536). Springer Link. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-88538-0_67
  • Chen, M. (2012). Animacies: Biopolitics, racial mattering, and queer affect. Duke University Press. https://doi.org/10.1215/9780822395447
  • Chouinard, V. (2012). Mapping bipolar worlds: Lived geographies of "madness" in autobiographical accounts. Health and Place, 18(2), 144–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2011.08.009
  • Costley, D. M., Emerson, A., Ropar, D., & Sheppard, E. (2021). The anxiety caused by secondary schools for autistic adolescents: In their own words. Education Sciences, 11(11), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11110726
  • Costley, D. M., Emerson, A., Ropar, D., & Sheppard, E. (2023). Trying to solve the "worst situation" together: Participatory autism research. Educational Action Research, 31(4). 691–708. https://doi.org/10.1080/09650792.2021.2019075
  • Crenshaw, K. (1991). Mapping the margins: Intersectionality, identity politics, and violence against women of color. Stanford Law Review, 43(6), 1241–1299. https://doi.org/10.2307/1229039
  • Davidson, E. (2021). The child sensorium as privileged biopolitical resource: Sensory care and the burden of emotional control in middle class North American childhood. Environment and Planning C: Politics and Space, 39(6), 1129–1147. https://doi.org/10.1177/2399654419873926
  • Davidson, J. (2007). "In a world of her own…:" Re-presenting alienation and emotion in the lives and writings of women with autism. Gender, Place and Culture, 14(6), 659–677. https://doi.org/10.1080/09663690701659135
  • Davidson, J. (2008). Autistic culture online: Virtual communication and cultural expression on the spectrum. Social and Cultural Geography, 9(7), 791–806. https://doi.org/10.1080/14649360802382586
  • Davidson, J., & Henderson, V. L. (2010a). "Coming out" on the spectrum: Autism, identity and disclosure. Social and Cultural Geography, 11(2), 155–170. https://doi.org/10.1080/14649360903525240
  • Davidson, J., & Henderson, V. L. (2010b). "Travel in parallel with us for a while:" Sensory geographies of autism. Canadian Geographer / Le Géographe Canadien, 54(4), 462–475. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1541-0064.2010.00309.x
  • Davidson, J., & Smith, M. (2009). Autistic autobiographies and more-than-human emotional geographies. Environment and Planning D: Society and Space, 27(5), 898–916. https://doi.org/10.1068/d4308
  • Davidson, J., & Tamas, S. (2016). Autism and the ghost of gender. Emotion, Space and Society, 19, 59–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.emospa.2015.09.009
  • Dickman, M. C. (2022). Reproducing fatness and disability: Risk avoidance and the womb. Fat Studies: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Body Weight and Society, 11(1), 57–69. https://doi.org/10.1080/21604851.2021.1906527
  • Douglas, P. (2010). "Problematising" inclusion: Education and the question of autism. Pedagogy, Culture and Society, 18(2), 105–121. https://doi.org/10.1080/14681366.2010.488039
  • England, M. R. (2016). Being open in academia: A personal narrative of mental illness and disclosure. Canadian Geographer / Le Géographe Canadien, 60(2), 226–231. https://doi.org/10.1111/cag.12270
  • Erevelles, N., & Minear, A. (2010). Unspeakable offenses: Untangling race and disability in discourses of intersectionality. Journal of Literary & Cultural Disability Studies, 4(2), 127–145. https://doi.org/10.3828/jlcds.2010.11
  • Felepchuk, E. (2021). Stimming, improvisation, and COVID-19: (Re)negotiating autistic sensory regulation during a pandemic. Disability Studies Quarterly, 41(3). https://doi.org/10.18061/dsq.v41i3.8426
  • Foucault, M. (1977). Discipline and punish: The birth of the prison. Penguin.
  • Garland-Thomson, R. (2011). Misfits: A feminist materialist disability concept. Hypatia, 26(3), 591–609. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1527-2001.2011.01206.x
  • Graby, S. (2015). Neurodiversity: Bridging the gap between the disabled people's movement and the mental health system survivors' movement? In H. Spandler, J. Anderson, & B. Sapey (Eds.), Madness, distress and the politics of disablement (pp. 231-244). Policy Press Online. https://doi.org/10.1332/policypress/9781447314578.001.0001
  • Grant, A., & Kara, H. (2021). Considering the autistic advantage in qualitative research: The strengths of autistic researchers. Contemporary Social Science, 16(5), 589–603. https://doi.org/10.1080/21582041.2021.1998589
  • Grinker, R. R. (2020). Autism, "stigma," disability: A shifting historical terrain. Current Anthropology, 61(S21), 555–567. https://doi.org/10.1086/705748
  • Hart, B. (2014). Autism parents and neurodiversity: Radical translation, joint embodiment and the prosthetic environment. BioSocieties, 9(3), 284–303. https://doi.org/10.1057/biosoc.2014.20
  • Henderson, V. L., Davidson, J., Hemsworth, K., & Edwards, S. (2014). Hacking the master code: Cyborg stories and the boundaries of autism. Social and Cultural Geography, 15(5), 504–524. https://doi.org/10.1080/14649365.2014.898781
  • Hilton, L. J. (2017). Avonte's Law: Autism, wandering, and the racial surveillance of neurological difference. African American Review, 50(2), 221–235. https://doi.org/10.1353/afa.2017.0023
  • Jack, J. (2014). Autism and gender: From refrigerator mothers to computer geeks. University of Illinois Press. https://doi.org/10.5406/illinois/9780252038372.001.0001
  • Jaysane-Darr, A. (2020). Enabling and disabling emotional diversity: Negotiating autism spectrum disorder in therapeutic encounters. Children & Society, 34(4), 261–275. https://doi.org/10.1111/chso.12374
  • Judge, S. M. (2018). Languages of sensing: Bringing neurodiversity into more-than-human geography. Environment and Planning D: Society and Space, 36(6), 1101–1119. https://doi.org/10.1177/0263775817748944
  • Kafer, A. (2013). Feminist, queer, crip. Indiana University Press.
  • Kapp, S. (Ed.). (2020). Autistic community and the neurodiversity movement: Stories from the frontline. Springer Link. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-8437-0
  • Kenna, T. (2022). Cities of neurodiversity: New directions for an urban geography of neurodiversity. Area, 54(4), 646-654. https://doi.org/10.1111/area.12803
  • Kitchin, R. (2020). Space and spatiality. In A. Kobayashi (Ed.), International encyclopedia of human geography (2nd ed., Vol. 12, pp. 321–327). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-102295-5.10998-9
  • Kline, W. (2001). Building a better race: Gender, sexuality, and eugenics from the turn of the century to the baby boom. University of California Press. https://doi.org/10.1525/9780520939318
  • Krazinski, M. (2023). Celebrating neurodivergence amid social injustice. Hypatia, 38(4), 726–745. https://doi.org/10.1017/hyp.2023.79
  • Lambert, R., & Harriss, E. (2022). Insider accounts of dyslexia from research mathematicians. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 111(1), 89-107. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-021-10140-2
  • Latour, B. (2004). How to talk about the body? The normative dimension of science studies. Body & Society, 10(2–3), 205–229. https://doi.org/10.1177/1357034X04042943
  • Latour, B. (2007). Reassembling the social: An introduction to actor-network-theory. Open University Press.
  • Lawson Jacobs, N. (2023) Speaking with us, not for us: Neurodiversity, theology and justice. Journal of Disability & Religion, 27(4), 584-605. https://doi.org/10.1080/23312521.2023.2249448
  • Lefebvre, H. (1991). The production of space. Wiley-Blackwell.
  • Lilley, R., Sedgwick, M., & Pellicano, E. (2020). Inclusion, acceptance, shame and isolation: Attitudes to autism in aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander communities in Australia. Autism, 24(7), 1860–1873. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361320928830
  • Malpas, J. (2012). Putting space in place: Philosophical topography and relational geography. Environment and Planning D: Society and Space, 30(2), 226–242. https://doi.org/10.1068/d20810
  • Manase, N. (2024). Africanising neurodiversity: A postcolonial view. In T. Chataika and D. Goodley (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of postcolonial disability studies (pp. 103-113). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003310709-12
  • McGuire, A. (2016). War on autism: On the cultural logic of normative violence. University of Michigan Press. https://doi.org/10.3998/mpub.7784427
  • McRuer, R., & Johnson, M. L. (2014). Proliferating cripistemologies: A virtual roundtable. Journal of Literary & Cultural Disability Studies, 8(2), 149–169. https://doi.org/10.3828/jlcds.2014.12
  • Meerai, S., Abdillahi, I., & Poole, J. (2016). An introduction to anti-Black sanism. Intersectionalities: A global journal of social work analysis, research, polity, and practice, 5(3), 18–35. https://doi.org/10.48336/IJFXWF8702
  • Mitchell, D., & Snyder, S. (2015). The biopolitics of disability: Neoliberalism, ablenationalism, and peripheral embodiment. University of Michigan Press. https://doi.org/10.3998/mpub.7331366
  • Moten, F. (2009). The case of Blackness. Criticism, 50(2), 177–218. https://doi.org/10.1353/crt.0.0062
  • Nash, J. C. (2017). Intersectionality and its discontents. American Quarterly, 69(1), 117–129. https://doi.org/10.1353/aq.2017.0006
  • North, Gemma. (2021). Reconceptualising "reasonable adjustments" for the successful employment of autistic women. Disability & Society, 38(6), 944-962. https://doi.org/10.1080/09687599.2021.1971065
  • Oliver, M. (1990). The politics of disablement. Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-20895-1
  • Oswin, N., & Olund, E. (2010). Governing intimacy. Environment and Planning D: Society and Space, 28(1), 60–67. https://doi.org/10.1068/d2801ed
  • Pickens, T. A. (2019). Black madness: Mad Blackness. Duke University Press. https://doi.org/10.1215/9781478005506
  • Pluquailec, J. (2018). Affective economies, autism, and "challenging behaviour:" Socio-spatial emotions in disabled children's education. Emotion, Space and Society, 29, 9–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.emospa.2018.07.004
  • Povinelli, E. A. (2011). Economies of abandonment: Social belonging and endurance in late liberalism. Duke University Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9780822394570
  • Puar, J. K. (2017). The right to maim: Debility, capacity, disability. Duke University Press. https://doi.org/10.1215/9780822372530
  • Russell, G. (2020). Critiques of the neurodiversity movement. In S. K. Kapp (Ed.), Autistic community and the neurodiversity movement: Stories from the frontline (pp. 287–303). Palgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-8437-0_21
  • Saldanha, A. (2006). Reontologising race: The machinic geography of phenotype. Environment and Planning D: Society and Space, 24(1), 9–24. https://doi.org/10.1068/d61j
  • Savarese, R. J. (2018). See It Feelingly: Classic Novels, Autistic Readers, and the Schooling of a No-Good English Professor. Duke University Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9781478093473
  • Seers, K., & Hogg, R. C. (2021). "You don't look autistic:" A qualitative exploration of women's experiences of being the "autistic other." Autism, 25(6), 1553–1564. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361321993722
  • Sewell, A., & Park, J. (2021). A three-factor model of educational practice considerations for teaching neurodiverse learners from a strengths-based perspective. Support for Learning, 36(4), 678–694. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9604.12387
  • Shiels, T., Kenny, N., Shiels, R., & Mannix-McNamara, P. (2021). Incivility in higher education: Challenges of inclusion for neurodiverse students with traumatic brain injury in Ireland. Societies, 11(2), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc11020060
  • Shoorcheh, M. (2019). On the spatiality of geographic knowledge. Asian Geographer, 36(1), 63–80. https://doi.org/10.1080/10225706.2018.1463854
  • Simpson, J., Yantzi, N., Shute, T., & Watson, S. (2024). "Even if it's flawed it's still beautiful:" Life lessons learned by adolescents with neurological conditions at summer camp. Disability & Society, 39(1), 16-39. https://doi.org/10.1080/09687599.2021.1994372
  • Sinclair, J. (2010). Cultural commentary: Being autistic together. Disability Studies Quarterly, 30(1). https://doi.org/10.18061/dsq.v30i1.1075
  • Solomon, O., & Lawlor, M. C. (2018). Beyond V40.31: Narrative phenomenology of wandering in autism and dementia. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry, 42(2), 206–243. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11013-017-9562-7
  • Thompson, V. E. (2021). Policing in Europe: Disability justice and abolitionist intersectional care. Race & Class, 62(3), 61–76. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306396820966463
  • Tomlinson, E., & Newman, S. (2017). Valuing writers from a neurodiversity perspective: Integrating new research on autism spectrum disorder into composition pedagogy. Composition Studies, 45(2), 91–112. https://www.jstor.org/stable/26402785
  • Udonsi, P. (2022). Young, gifted and black: the intersectionality of race, intellectual disability and neurodivergence. Critical and Radical Social Work, 10(2), 226–241. https://doi.org/10.1332/204986021X16530492120870
  • von Below, R., Spaeth, E., & Horlin, C. (2024). Autism in higher education: Dissonance between educators' perceived knowledge and reported teaching behaviour. International Journal of Inclusive Education, 28(6), 940-957. https://doi.org/10.1080/13603116.2021.1988159
  • Walker, N. (2021). Neuroqueer heresies: Notes on the neurodiversity paradigm, autistic empowerment, and postnormal possibilities. Autonomous Press.
  • Walker, N., & Raymaker, D. (2021). Toward a neuroqueer future: An interview with Nick Walker. Autism in Adulthood, 3(1), 5–10. https://doi.org/10.1089/aut.2020.29014.njw
  • Waltz, M. (2013). Autism: A social and medical history. Palgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137328533
  • Waltz, M. (2020). The production of the "normal" child: Neurodiversity and the commodification of parenting. In H. Bertilsdotter Rosqvist, N. Chown, & A. Stenning (Eds.), Neurodiversity studies: A new critical paradigm (pp. 15–26). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429322297-3
  • Wilson, J. (2017). Reimagining disability and inclusive education through universal design for learning. Disability Studies Quarterly, 37(2). https://doi.org/10.18061/dsq.v37i2.5417
  • Yergeau, M. R. (2018). Authoring autism: On rhetoric and neurological queerness. Duke University Press. https://doi.org/10.1215/9780822372189
  • Yergeau, M. R. (2020). Cassandra isn't doing the robot: On risky rhetorics and contagious autism. Rhetoric Society Quarterly, 50(3), 212–221. https://doi.org/10.1080/02773945.2020.1752132

Endnotes

  1. This phrasing can be misread, as Latour rejects the distinction between body and object for relying on a "parasitic" ontology of an active subject and a passive object (Latour, 2004, p. 207). In Latour's ontology, what we deem "objects" are actually bodies along with what/whom we deem subjects.
    Return to Text
  2. Mary Dickman (2022) identifies another intimate space at risk of producing autism, as well as ADHD. Fat people's wombs have been biopolitically framed as high risk of producing neurodivergence.
    Return to Text
  3. Many autistics suffer PTSD due to their experiences with APA and argue for its abolition. For a brief overview of the debate, see Chapman and Bovell (2022).
    Return to Text
Return to Top of Page