Descriptions of autism in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manuals of the APA and throughout much of the history of autism emphasize difficulties in social interaction, communication and imaginative play. Recent reports by self-advocates, neuroscientists and other researchers suggest that sensory and movement differences may play a significant part in the lives of those who live with autism. Sensory and movement differences may include difficulties in starting, stopping, continuing, combining and switching motor action, speech, thought, memory and emotion. A review of these reports, the experience of those with other movement differences, and implications for understanding individuals with autism are presented. Suggestions are included on how knowledge of sensory and movement differences may offer guidance in rethinking assumptions about autism characteristics, social interactions, communication and other supports.
Authorship is considered equal. The authors wish to thank Dr. Gail Evra for her invaluable editorial assistance.
I was intensely preoccupied with the movement of the spinning coin or lid and I saw nothing and heard nothing. I did it because it shut out sound that hurt my ears. No sound intruded on my fixation. It was like being deaf. Even a sudden noise didn't startle me out of my world.
People labeled with autism often move their bodies in ways that are unfamiliar to us. Some people rock, repeatedly touch an object, jump and finger posture while other people come to a standstill in a doorway, sit until cued to move, or turn away when someone beckons. As professionals trained to see these as autistic behaviors, most of us have interpreted such movements as both volitional and meaningless; or as communicative acts signaling avoidance of interaction and evidence of diminished cognitive capacity; or as some combination of these, and often have targeted them for reduction. We have taken a socially constructed interpretation of what we see and have built a "theory" of autism.
This paper challenges the traditional definitions of autism that give primacy to a triad of deficits in social interaction, communication and imaginative play (Wing, 1981; Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV (DSM-IV) (APA, 2000). The approach is both widely known and essentially unchallenged despite broad acknowledgement that autism is a condition that reflects some differences in a person's neurology. Typically, the neurological implications have not become part of the description. Over the past two decades, however, researchers and self-advocates have begun to rethink this socially defined focus. They express concern that children and adults with the autism label may be challenged by unrecognized and significant sensory and movement differences (e.g. Hill & Leary 1993; Williams, 1993; Bristol, Cohen, Costello, Denckla, Eckberg, Kallen, Kraemer, Lord, Maurer, McIlvane, Minshew, Sigman, & Spence, 1996; Donnellan & Leary, 1995; Leary & Hill, 1996; Filipek et al., 2000; Donnellan, 2001; Sullivan, 2002; Dhossche, 2004; Bluestone, 2005; Nayate, Bradshaw & Rinehart, 2005; Endow, 2006; Jansiewicz, Goldberg, Newschaffer, Denkla, Landa & Mostofsky, 2006; Mostofsky et al., 2006; Leekam, Nieto, Libby, Wing & Gould, 2007; Markram, Rinaldi & Markram, 2007; Tomchek & Dunn, 2007; Gernsbacher, Sauer, Geye,Schweigert, & Hill Goldsmith, 2008; Green, Charman, Pickles, Chandler, Loucas, Simonoff, & Baird, 2009; Goldman, Wang, Salgado, Greene, Kim & Rapin, 2009; and Mostofsky, Powell, Simmonds, Goldberg, Caffo, & Pekar, 2009).
Researchers and others describe these differences using a variety of terms, such as motor problems, sensory-integration problems, inertia, sensory overload, apraxia, dyspraxia, echolalia, mutism, behavior disorder, catatonia, or clumsiness. To reflect the range and complexity of sensory perception and movement related phenomena, we use the term "sensory and movement differences" as it encompasses the dynamic interaction of sensation and movement (Gibson, 1979; Thelen & Smith, 1995) while acknowledging that many differences are merely part of the richness of human diversity.
Behavior is highly interpretable. Some behaviors may be communicative; some may be volitional. Some behaviors, however, may not be intentional. Rather, observed behaviors may be artifacts of the difficulties a person may be having in organizing and regulating sensation and movement. Still others may be subtle signals of the desire for relationship or expressions of meaning. Therapeutic and intervention-based approaches, designed to address perceived and identified challenging and problematic behaviors of individuals with autism, tend to oversimplify the complex nature of human interactions in an attempt to delineate and manipulate variables contributing to and sustaining particular behaviors.
As we have professionalized interactions with people with autism, we have trained professionals, parents and others to interpret what happens in terms of simple, binary views of behavior (i.e. good/bad or positive/negative), and to see behavior as controlled by immediate, situational antecedents and consequences. When we focus on these socially constructed expectations for behavior and communication in our fast-paced, super technological world, we miss opportunities to know and understand people who may experience their existence and interactions in very different ways. Behaviors may not be what they seem to be (Donnellan, Leary & Robledo, 2006).
Our interest in the topic of sensory and movement differences has grown from reports by many self-advocates with the autism label and their caregivers that disturbances of sensation and movement are a constant concern, frequently constraining the ability to communicate, relate to others and participate in life (e.g., Strandt-Conroy, 1999; Barron & Barron, 1992; Rubin, Biklen, Kasa-Hendrickson; Kluth, Cardinal, & Broderick, 2001). Organizing and regulating sensory information and movement in order to participate in social relationships may be frustrating for people with such differences. These differences can involve difficulties initiating and executing movements or difficulties with stopping, combining, and switching sensation and movement, including speech, thought and emotion (Hill & Leary, 1993; Donnellan & Leary, 1995; Donnellan, Leary & Robledo, 2006), making social relationships and many other activities very challenging and even overwhelming.
Self-advocates also report that they lack sensation or feedback from their bodies and may feel physically unaware of their facial expressions, position in space and movements (e.g., Blackman, 1999; Hale & Hale, 1999; Williams, 1996a, 1996b, 2003). Some experience the sights and sounds of their world as being painfully intense (Condon, 1985; Williams, 1992 & 1996b; Markram, Rinaldi & Markram, 2007). Extreme emotions can cause the individual to become stuck, unable to initiate or cease repetition of a movement. Self confidence and reputation often suffer when others assume a person is repeating an action "on purpose." Sean Barron wrote, "All I wanted was to be like the other kids my age. It felt as if I was weird and strange on the outside, but inside I wasn't like that. The inside person wanted to get out and break free of all the behaviors that I was a slave to and couldn't stop" (Barron & Barron, 1992, p. 181). For many people, as for Sean, simple movements can lead to repetitions or perseveration, even when they want to stop the movement.
Our concern here is not to discard useful information already accumulated via a primarily socially defined approach to autism. Nor are we interested in enhancing a deficit-based approach to understanding autism, or in creating a new disability category. We do not propose to specify a cause of autism or a site of lesion or dysfunction within the central nervous system. Rather, we write to share our emerging awareness that people may struggle with difficulties that are not immediately evident to an outsider. That is, our experience of individuals with autism ought no longer to be assumed the same as their experience. Individuals with the autism label often describe experiences which are not immediately obvious to the rest of us but which may well affect our understanding of their behavior. These experiences frequently fit the definition of sensory and movement differences. Sue Rubin (August 4, 2007 personal communication) described her dilemma with intention and action: "When you said we could stay and asked dad to do the shopping for the Asperger's barbeque, my body relaxed and autism let me eat the melon." And two other autistic adults had the following interaction about sensory and movement differences. Judy Endow (personal communication on Facebook, January 25, 2009) described her experiences in relation to sensory and movement differences as follows:
I think the fluidity of access to various places in my brain is dependent upon neurological movement between places. I'm no scientist, but have always been able to "see" this inside of me. Sometimes my speaking is hindered, other times my thinking, and sometimes my physical movement. The hardest is when thinking is not working smoothly. When that happens, I have to line up one thought at a time, like train cars. I like it much better when my thoughts do not have to be methodically lined up, but are more fluid with colors coming in and out and swirling into unique and beautiful patterns. (My thoughts are in pictures and sometimes moving colors).
Phil Schwarz (personal communication on Facebook, January 25, 2009) commented on Judy's description by using another analogy:
I think that processing bandwidth — what Judy calls "neurological movement between places" — is a critical factor in autism. I think that those of us who learn to cope develop adaptations that allow more parsimonious use of the bandwidth available to us: love of sameness, or of patterns, or of predictability (so that we can apply the bandwidth we do have to *deviations* from the predicted or from the patterns). There is a coherent autistic aesthetic sensibility, that is informed by this search for parsimony of bandwidth use, and for titration of excesses.
This paper explores some of the implications of sensory and movement differences in the development and experiences of individuals with the autism label. We note, of course, that some researchers and clinicians completely deny the possibility that individuals with autism might experience any problems with movement. Rimland (1993), a psychologist long a proponent of a biological approach to autism, wrote the following:
It has been widely recognized for many decades that the vast majority of autistic persons are quite unimpaired with regard to their finger dexterity and gross motor capabilities. They have in fact often been described as especially dexterous and coordinated. The literature abounds with stories of young autistic children who can take apart and reassemble small mechanical devices, build towers of blocks and dominos higher than a normal adult can, assemble jigsaw puzzles and climb to dangerously high places without falling. The files of the Autism Research Institute contain over 17,000 questionnaires completed by the parents of autistic children. Finger dexterity is one question we've asked about since 1965. Most parents indicate that their children are average or above in the use of their hands. The idea that autism is, or typically involves, a "movement disorder" is simply ludicrous. (p. 3)
Likewise, Mulick, Jacobson & Kobe (1993), behavioral psychologists, stated unequivocally that clinical experience argues against any motor/movement difficulties, particularly voluntary control of movement as in apraxia:
Scientific evidence for developmental apraxia in autism is lacking. Autistic youngsters are often characterized by better-developed motor skills than verbal skills, even real non-verbal problem solving talent… There is no research evidence at all to support the position that people with autism experience such global problems. The usual clinical finding, familiar to any psychologist who routinely works in this area, is that motor impairment and delay is much less common than communication disorder and delay (Jacobson & Ackerman, 1990, p.274). (Italics in original)
This common approach to autism pays scant attention to possible somatic difficulties resulting from neurological differences. Perhaps, this is a function of the dominance of psychology and psychiatry for the first 50 or more years of the autism story. Yet some psychologists and psychiatrists did report movement differences and even catatonic symptoms in autism long before Rimland or Mulick et al. and others denied the existence of such evidence (e.g. Damasio & Maurer, 1978; Wing & Attwood, 1987). More recently, many researchers have noted the presence of impairments in basic motor skills: gait, posture, balance, speed, coordination (e.g., Ghaziuddin & Butler, 1998; Jansiewicz et al., 2006; Noterdaeme et al., 2002; Rinehart et al., 2006).
Many neuroscientists now are stressing the significance and implications of motor and sensory difficulties in the development of children with autism. For example, Sutera, et al. (2007) looked at four year-olds who had been diagnosed at age two and received early intervention of various amounts and types. Of particular interest were the children who "lost" the diagnosis of autism by age four. Sutera, et al. found that the best predictor of this outcome for very young children with autism is motor skill at age two. Mostofsky (2008) noted this finding and addressed concerns about the exclusion of motor problems from the "core" features of autism in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, (APA, 2000) "…despite [an] abundance of literature suggesting otherwise."
A growing number of researchers and clinicians in a broad range of disciplines continue to stress the importance of studying motor function in autism because, as Rogers and Benetto (2002) reported "….studies show that movement abnormalities are present early in children with autism, and may precede the emergence of the syndrome." Mostofsky noted: "Motor signs can serve as markers for deficits in parallel brain systems important for control of socialization and communication." For example, children with autism are often described as lacking reciprocity. Esther Thelen (1941-2004), an innovative researcher of infant development, upon reviewing the issue of motor development in autism asked: "How can you talk about "reciprocity" or lack thereof as a psychological phenomenon if the child has motor problems?" (1997 Personal Communication)
In the course of development, if individuals move and respond in idiosyncratic ways from infancy, they will experience all interactions within a unique frame that most certainly differs from that which is called typical. The cumulative effect of such interactions will be one in which all aspects of relationships, including how to establish and maintain them, may be markedly skewed from the broader cultural consensus and expected rules of how relationships work. (See Stern, 2005; Gibson, 1979; Thelen & Smith, 1995 for reviews of the complex and dynamic interrelationship of movement, perception, relationship and cognitive development.) Our experience and self-advocate reports have taught us that individuals with autism often are aware of their idiosyncrasies, may not be able to control them but do want communication, participation and relationship. In order to make this possible, we need to acknowledge and accommodate the differences so that communication, relationship and participation can happen.
Dynamic Interactions of Nervous System, Body and Environment
As we have noted elsewhere (Donnellan, Leary & Robledo, 2006), the writings of many authors interested in movement describe a unity of perception, action, emotion, and thought. Moshe Feldenkrais (1904-1984), a physicist, martial artist, and renowned movement practitioner noted: "Our self-image consists of four components that are involved in every action: movement, sensation, feeling and thought" (Feldenkrais, 1972, p. 10). His method is two-fold and may involve independent "awareness through movement" exercises and more hands-on "functional integration" guided movement exercises with the physical assistance of a trained practitioner.
Likewise, in his fascinating book, Awakenings, Sacks (1990) wrote of the self-reports of his patients with post-encephalitic Parkinson's disease who temporarily "awoke" through the use of the drug L-Dopa. They all had been sick from the same disease, encephalitis lethargica. The area of damage in the brains caused by the disease was clearly established. Nonetheless, each developed his or her own personalized version of movement disorder, and many of their difficulties were unknown to the medical staff until the patients became able to speak. The variety of manifestations of symptoms encompassed difficulties with many hidden aspects of human experience: perception of the passing of time; interest in normal activities; fatigue; memory; and recurring thoughts.
Thelen incorporated dynamic systems models in her innovative research on movement in child development (Thelen & Smith 1994; Thelen, 1995). In this view, perceptions, movement, thoughts, and emotions can be linked together by having coincidentally (and possibly routinely) co-occurred. Experience may selectively reinforce them as a bundle. They can be unbundled or softly assembled as required by the context. The individual is always operating within an environment or context and, as the context changes, systems scan, adjust, and shift as necessary to meet new demands. These contextual shifts play a vital role in movement. Context comes together in such a way as to allow the movement to emerge or not; a movement and, indeed, the person or persons are part of the context (Thelen & Smith, 1994). As Bateson (1972) told us years ago, context is far more than what is left when we take out the part we wish to study.
No single component is causal in determining the movement. As these are dynamic systems, the components are the context that determine the product. Thelen & Smith (1994) further explained that "...even behaviors that look wired in or program-driven can be seen as dynamically emergent: behavior is assembled by the nature of the task, and opportunistically recruits the necessary and available organic components (which themselves have dynamic histories) and environmental support" (p. 73). These may be actions, thoughts, words, memories or sense experiences. Recall Proust, where the smell of a cookie released the hundreds of pages of Remembrances of Things Past.
Thelan's approach offers new ways to understand the inconsistent abilities and disabilities of individuals with the autism label. Speech is an example of dynamic behavior. Speech is not lost or gained; it emerges when all necessary components recruited, and appropriately regulated and organized, allow its production. Stress often makes speech difficult or even impossible. And stress need not be negative; excitement may also cause difficulties. Paradoxically, for some people with sensory and movement differences, stress also may help produce speech. While presenting with the authors at an Autism Society of America conference in July 1996, Arthur Shawlow, Nobel laureate in physics and father of an adult son with autism, reported that his son could say a complete, and original, context-appropriate sentence about once every eight to ten years. He asked the audience how many parents had similar experiences and at least 18 sets of parents raised their hands. They met and compared notes. Most of the labeled children of these individuals were able to speak under extreme, often negative, circumstances. Some had only spoken once or twice in a lifetime.
Reports of this kind are not unusual in the sensory and movement differences literature, among the autism community, or in our own 100+ years of combined experience with children and adults with the autism label. More common are phenomena such as echolalia, mutism, speech uttered only under unique circumstances, e.g. speaking what they have written. In the dynamic system model, the notion of emergence begins to give us a way to understand and perhaps support people with these differences. Strandt-Conroy (1999) compiled 40 hours of interviews with adults with autism who experienced such symptoms and more. Her interviews had to be adjusted to the specialized needs of the interviewees. Several could only answer written questions sent in advance; others if they were on the phone and in a warm bath. Likewise, the autistic people in Robledo & Donnellan (2007) each had personalized supports to enable them to participate in the interviews. We refer to these specialized arrangements as accommodations after Luria (1932) and Sacks (1990). We define accommodations as adjustments or adaptations of an interaction, a task, situation, or the environment that assist a person to temporarily get around difficulties organizing and regulating sensory information or movement (see Donnellan, Leary & Robledo, 2006 for examples).
Learning from Neurological Symptoms in Other Sensory and Movement Disorders
In our review of the history of movement differences, we found early descriptions of catatonia in the work of Kahlbaum, (1874/1973) which seemed startlingly familiar (see Hill & Leary, 1993; Donnellan & Leary,1995; Starkstein, Goldar & Hodgkiss,1995; Leary & Hill, 1996). In the nineteenth century there was no clear distinction between neurological and psychiatric symptoms. As the two fields diverged in the early twentieth century, however, some conditions gravitated into one or the other. Catatonia is presently defined as a characteristic of certain kinds of schizophrenia, though many have argued over the years for a more neurological view of the disorder (Rogers, 1992; Abrams & Taylor, 1976). The discussion of where to place catatonia and catatonic symptoms is once again topical because of the plan to update the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the APA, Some, in fact, are arguing for the inclusion of catatonia as a separate diagnostic category or under "movement disturbances" (Taylor & Fink, 2003; Fink & Taylor, 2006; Penland, Weder & Tampi, 2006; Caroff & Ungvari, 2007;). Irrespective of that discussion, it is useful to look at the symptoms described by Kahlbaum and other early and recent authors as these may illuminate the symptoms seen in individuals with autism and other developmental disabilities.
In Table 1, the characteristic features and symptoms on the left side of the table are borrowed from descriptors specific to several kinds of movement disorders, (Kalbaum, 1874; Fink & Taylor, 2006; Caroff and Ungvari, 2007; http://www.movementdisorders.org/disorders, 2009). The list of movement disorders symptoms is not in any particular order or hierarchy; rather, symptoms are listed randomly as taken from the above literature sources. The intent here is to show the scope of symptoms by feature that may account for certain behaviors seen in autism. Examples of behaviors listed on the right side of Table 1 appear there because they have been discussed in a previously published review of the autism literature and movement disturbances (Leary & Hill 1996). The majority of these have also been documented and observed throughout many years of clinical practice with a large number of individuals with autism across the life span.
Movement Disturbance Feature | Symptom Evidence In Autism |
---|---|
Repetitive motor actions | e.g., Tapping, touching, grimacing |
Rhythmical, cyclical movements | e.g., Rocking, shrugging, squinting, pouting |
Lack of Initiation | Requires prompts and cues to perform |
Difficulty imitating other's actions | Both immediate and delayed motor imitation difficulties |
Echophenomena | Mimesis; elaborate copying of others actions — verbal and/or motor |
Immobility | Remains fixed and inert in position and posture for extended time periods |
Withdrawal | Isolates self away from focal activity and others |
Grimacing | Facial/oral-motor movements |
Stereotypies | Repetitive movements of the hands, limbs extremities and whole body |
Aversion | Of eye gaze and attention to other |
Negativism | Oppositional actions elicited with passive movement and overall behavior |
Automatic obedience; Suggestibility | Extreme compliance in response to verbal suggestion and environmental cues |
Rigidity | Muscles rigid to passive movement |
Bradykinesia | Slowness of movements, feebleness |
Tremor | Essential, intentional, rest, postural etc. |
Forced grasping | Of another's hands, wrists, etc., or items in the environment |
Akinesia | Marked absence of action and movements |
Akathisia | Motor restlessness, moves about but not goal-directed |
Ataxia | Loss of coordination in motor action execution |
Perseveration | Motor or other repeated behavior after being elicited an initial stimulus |
Ambitendency | Appears "stuck" in indecisive, hesitant movements |
Tics | Motor and/or verbal |
Obstruction; Blocking | Incomplete movement towards a goal — "gets stuck" en route to goal |
Difficulty with stopping, cessation of movement | Will continue movements unless redirected or stopped by an external means |
Mannerisms | Uses intact and entire motor action sequences out of context e.g., salutes |
Waxy flexibility | Automatic ease and compliance with assuming unusual postures for extended time |
Ballismus | Violent, rapid and apparently involuntary actions and movements |
Choreiform movements | Rapid and apparently involuntary travelling and "dancing" ripples of movement |
Catalepsy (posturing) | Maintains seemingly uncomfortable and imposed postures for extended time |
Athetoid movements | Slow, writhing movements and actions |
Spasms | Muscular spasms of varying durations affecting muscle groups |
Dystonias | Sustained torsion due to muscle contractions in varied muscle groups |
Impulsivity | Actions and movements triggered suddenly |
Self-injury, mutilation | Disturbing and persistent attempts to inflict pain on self |
Excitement; Frenzy | Marked episodes of extreme amounts of activity for extended time |
Aggression, Destruction | Unprecipitated violent actions directed to others and the environment |
Stupor | Prolonged period of total immobility, lack of responsiveness and mutism |
Rituals | Object-related actions on objects as part of a routine, repeated event |
Motility changes | e.g. Toe walking, skipping, hopping |
Changes in speech behavior | e.g., Mutism; question repetition,; echolalia; verbigeration; logorrhoea; foreign accent; changes in prosody; difficulty modulating volume |
Autonomic changes | Changes in typical autonomic functions e.g., heart rate, perspiration, breathing, core body temperature |
Leary and Hill (1996) analyzed the literature on symptoms associated with established movement disorders and those associated with autism. The greatest difference among these disabilities was the interpretation of the symptoms. In Tourette syndrome, Parkinson's disorder and catatonia, there was a neurological interpretation of symptoms. A social rather than a neurological interpretation was applied if the person had a label of autism. That which is called a "tic" in a person with Tourette syndrome is most often assumed to be a 'behavior' (and often a conscious choice) in a person with autism. For symptoms interpreted through a neurological lens, individuals tend to be appropriately supported. In autism, symptoms are viewed frequently as behaviors to be reduced or eliminated, often with a negative intervention and results. Table 2 illustrates descriptions given to similar behaviors dependent on a person's diagnosis.
Neurological terms | Social Interpretation of behavior |
---|---|
Akinesia | Non-compliance, social indifference |
Festination | Behavior excess, careless |
Bradykinesia | Lazy, slow |
Bradyphrenia | Mental retardation |
Tics | Aberrant behavior |
Obsessions/ Adventitious behaviors | Autistic behavior, 'stims' |
The sensory and movement differences reported by and observed in individuals with autism may have a significant impact on their and our ability to relate and participate in social interactions. A neurological view of symptoms possibly affecting autistic individuals will help us to understand further the nature of differences experienced by these individuals. While the psychological impact is very real as experienced first-hand by participants in such interactions, it is useful to suspend social interpretations of the symptoms so as not to mistakenly ascribe intent and volition to individuals whose behavior may be contrary to what really is intended and able to be communicated.
Detailed personal descriptions of movement and sensory differences found in other disabilities have given us some additional insight as to what it may be like for a person to deal with various symptoms, such as compelling impulses, a loss of conscious control, lack of initiation, akinetic moments and unusual ways of being in the world (e.g. McGoon, 1994). Frequently, the person has both the challenge of the movement difference and burden of blame and misunderstanding. In Strandt-Conroy's (1999) research, it was often necessary to use vignettes from people with other sensory and movement differences to enable the autistic interviewees to recognize their own experience. Most expressed gratitude for the opportunity to learn about movement differences as they often had blamed themselves for their behavior and thought they were alone in having these difficulties.
Implications of Sensory and Movement Differences for Understanding People Labeled with Autism
A Different Kind of Science
Woe to that science whose methods are developed in advance of its problems, so that the experimenter can see only those phases of a problem for which a method is already at hand.
We have stressed the neurological aspects of what are commonly thought of as autistic characteristics and behavior problems. We do not intend, however, to either suggest a whole new category of disabilities in autism nor to eliminate the psychological aspects. The issues here are similar to the challenges faced by those interested in Tourette syndrome. The syndrome was elucidated before the fields of neurology and psychiatry diverged (Gilles de la Tourette, 1885). For many years, psychiatry dominated the discussion and the treatment. In the past few decades, there has been a far greater emphasis on the neurology of the disorder. Yet, it is clear that it is not possible to separate the neurological from the psychological in a living human being. As Sacks suggests (1989) there is need for a different kind of science that views the individual as a whole person, mind and body. This shift has begun in Tourette syndrome. In addition, dynamic systems models of development suggest an emphasis on the unique history and the critical importance of context on the manifestations of the symptoms. Perhaps the present emphasis on discrete "autistic" behaviors tied to specific interventions should be seen in terms of more conscribed value and utility.
Developmental versus Acquired Symptoms
In addition to the personalized nature of the characteristics and the dynamic nature of the manifestations of a movement difference mentioned above, it is impossible to overemphasize the importance of the developmental aspects of movement differences in autism vs. adult acquired disorders. For example, bradykinesia, or very slow movements, might have a wide range of effects on adults with acquired disorders such as Parkinsonism. In an infant or a toddler, the possible effects of slow responding or delayed initiating would surely have an effect on the entire trajectory of development even if the difference were intermittent or barely perceptible to the parents or professionals. Of course, we are not suggesting that these autistic people have Parkinson's syndrome; rather, that they report sensory and movement differences which are not obvious to their caregivers, particularly parents of young children. Yet, the potential changes to the "dance of relationships" (Stern, 2000) alone would be worthy of many dissertations in child development. Still, the complexity of the task ought not deter us from attempting such inquiry because it could have enormous implications for our understanding of human development and diversity.
Interpretation of Symptoms as Volitional
Many of us have accepted without question the implicit message that unusual movements presented by people with autism are always volitional and often pleasurable. Symptoms of sensory and movement difference in autism are consistently interpreted by others as autistic behaviors. Neurological symptoms, such as sudden, loud vocalizations; being in constant motion; extreme response to minor changes; unusual mannerisms and gait; and "unmotivated" laughter are examples of behaviors commonly thought to be performed "on purpose" and targeted for behavioral intervention. A social interpretation of these symptoms leaves people with the assumption that they occur as a matter of choice, apathy, or learned behavior. Aggression during an episode of catatonic frenzy is viewed differently if the neurological aspects of the person's experience are considered. Typically, reprimands or contingent praise would not be used to change a recognized neurological symptom. As noted, the non-volitional aspects of behavior are rarely considered for people with autism. For example, the authors have all too often heard criticism and disparaging descriptions such as lazy or non-compliant applied to a person with autism who is in a non-responsive state. Frequently, the difficulty is related to stress, even the stress of excitement. An all too typical example is staff or family reporting that the child or adult refused to get out of the car or van to go to a place he or she seems to like. Intervention or support that is based on our social interpretations of symptoms may not always be helpful. Returning the non-compliant person to home, school or program usually results in additional trouble. We need a clearer understanding of people's experiences if we are to provide appropriate care and support that boosts self-confidence and is the product of collaboration rather than control. Donnellan, Leary & Robledo, (2006) offer many suggestions for accommodations that may help people with autism deal with these situations.
Interpretation of Symptoms as Meaningless
Our assumptions about a person's intention or meaning directly influence the way we respond moment to moment, the relationships we form, and the support we give to people. When we label aspects of a person's behavior as meaningless, we may miss opportunities to extend learning and develop our relationships. Echolalia serves well as an illustration. In the early years of behavioral intervention for people with autism from 1960 to 1980, professionals assessing a child's communication abilities were to assume that echolalia was the "meaningless repetition of a word or word group just spoken by another person" (Fay, 1969, p. 39), a non-functional, undesirable and "sick" behavior of autism (Lovaas, 1966; Lovaas, Schreibman & Koegel, 1974), and a communication disorder to be extinguished through behavior modification (Lovaas, 1977). In the 1980s, the fine and detailed work of researchers such as Baltaxe & Simmons (1977), Prizant & Duchan (1981) and Prizant & Rydell (1984) began to influence our assumptions about the intentions of autistic speakers and the possible communicative functions of both immediate and delayed echolalia. Many people now understand that echolalia is neither always meaningless nor always meaningful; rather it serves a variety of pragmatic functions on a sociolinguistic continuum. Although sometimes not intentional, echolalia may be used intentionally by many who lack other strategies for communicating to maintain relationships, improve their comprehension of spoken language and to express meaning (Kanner, 1946). Acknowledgement of a person's efforts to accommodate, improvise and create meaning is a cause for celebration and an opportunity to improve communication and boost self esteem.
Interpretation of Symptoms as "Not Interested" in Relating or Communicating
People with autism often communicate, behave and participate in unique, very personal, perhaps idiosyncratic ways that require their partners to be more flexible and open than usual in interpreting meaning and intention. Differences in the way people are able to use their bodies and focus their attention leads many to assume that a person does not care to participate or communicate and does not desire relationship. These assumptions affect our expectations, the way we speak with them and the educational and social opportunities we offer to them. Under the "criterion of the least dangerous assumption" (Donnellan, 1984), it is safest to assume that relationships are critical to human beings for learning and development even if, and perhaps especially if, they have difficulties in these areas (Robledo, 2006; Fogel, 1993).
The Critical Importance of Relationship in Learning and Development
The past 40 years have witnessed the growth of a body of knowledge, approaches and intervention methodologies designed to address the needs of individuals with autism. Often the kinds of intervention strategies at our disposal are based on ideas and theories that conflict with each other. The content of interventions may be highly prescriptive or more loosely defined. Research can be cited in support of the efficacy of almost any kind of approach for at least some individuals in some situations. We struggle as well to explain and describe that quality within any intervention that works and leads to growth and development between the partners involved. Perhaps the essential factor underlying any successful intervention has been overlooked or at least not credited in the research. We propose, along with a growing number of investigators, that the undefined element is the presence and nature of the relationship between persons in any interaction.
The role of relationship in learning is the centerpiece of sociocultural psychology. Sociocultural psychology views human development as essentially embedded and significantly dependent on interactions with more able and skilled members of the existing cultural and social context. While most of us believe that learning is enhanced by a facilitative relationship with a more mature thinker, western psychology has only recently directed attention to the nature of that relationship. Lev Vygotsky (1896 1934) was a Russian psychologist whose work described and defined the role of relationship in human development. His work emphasized the notion that cognitive and specific skill development is the result of internalizing interactions with others within a relationship (Bodrova & Leong, 1996). Ylvisker and Feeney (1998) have translated Vygotskian theory into a support model that focuses on apprenticeship and collaboration between the person and another with more expertise in the areas where support is needed. The "tutor" provides collaborative mediation that is fine-tuned to the learner's changing needs for support to enable participation in meaningful, project-oriented work. "The roots of cognitive, executive and communication functions, as well as behavioral self-regulation, are everyday social interaction routines" (Ylvisker & Feeney,1998, pp. 15-16). In the sociocultural models of development, relationship with others serves as the springboard for learning. Learning happens within a social context, within a dialogue with others. We acquire cognitive skills, knowledge and behavior regulation, not simply through memorization of facts or actions, but through our interactions in the social world where this knowledge has function and meaning.
Inconsistency in Abilities
People report sensory and movement inconsistencies, such as fluctuations in speed and clarity of sensory perception; unreliable ability to maintain or release body postures; delays in speed and accuracy of movement and speech; unpredictable changes in muscle tone; unwanted vocal, verbal and physical tics and extraneous, non-functional movement (e.g. Mirenda & Donnellan, 1986; Williams, 1996a; Strandt-Conroy, 1999; Harp, 2008). A sensory and movement difference is characterized by this inconsistency, causing stress for the most common of movements (Baggs, 2007). A person struggling with these performance characteristics may not be able to predict, plan for, or sustain effective participation. For example, a person with a 14 second delay in her ability to respond to others (Mirenda & Donnellan, 1986) is likely to be misinterpreted and misunderstood and unlikely to be offered time to respond. This is illustrated by Harp (2008) on her blog, Asperger's Square 8 (used with permission).
Supporting Self Esteem
Humans carry inside themselves an image that includes reasons for, and the possibility of, change. We need to know that we are OK just as we are, even though there are things we may want to learn, or to do, better.
A current trend in early intervention for young children with autism is to provide guidance in massive quantities (e.g. 40 hours a week of one-to on-instruction). This guidance is naturally accompanied by frequent corrections and redirection. Given the intensity of this intervention, special care is needed to promote children's self-esteem at any age.
Equally important is the need for positive, optimistic, respectful support for adults with autism. The paucity of quality programs, diminished opportunity for interesting lives, effects of medication and chemical restraint are just a few of the additional burdens on these individuals and their families. Issues of collaboration, personalization and comfort are also essential for children and particularly pressing for the adult population with the autism label. McGinnity & Negri (1995) offer helpful suggestions on how students and staff can learn to be more sensitive to the differences in those on the autism spectrum.
Collaboration, Personalization and Comfort
The growth of the autism industry over the past two decades has spawned no end of books, interventions, programs and products. Yet, the diagnosis of autism is not prescriptive of the type of supports needed for assisting any particular person to participate, relate and communicate. Supports for people with autism should be personalized, reflect the respect and dignity due to all people, and address the challenges with which people struggle to organize and regulate themselves in response to the sensory environment and their movement differences. Appropriate supports require a deep and local knowledge of the individual. This can be gained from those who know and appreciate them but often such information is not available. Then it is even more essential to spend significant time with the person in a variety of activities and settings and with people who respect and admire him or her. We need to learn to listen with all of our senses and compassion (Savarese, 2007; Lovett, 1996) and to "presume competence" in all interactions. We do not put people in jeopardy by overestimating their experience. We do look for competence instead of deficits and talk to people in age appropriate ways. And we model such interactions for all those who are, or may become, willing to know them better.
Moreover, we need to remember that in our journey of change, we all need allies who will collaborate with us to find the most comfortable and effective ways for us to learn to participate in our families, with our friends and as contributing members of our communities (Schwarz, 2004; Robledo & Donnellan, 2008; Hill & Leary, in press). This is particularly critical for those persons who are challenged by the movement differences that often make such comfort temporary, personhood elusive, and collaboration a mystery. There is much to be learned from self-advocates with autism as well as from individuals who share some of the symptoms of movement differences such as Tourette syndrome, Parkinson's disorder and their supporters (e.g. Williams, 1992; McGoon, 1994). For example, individuals with Tourette syndrome have taught us that naming a behavior might make it much more difficult for a person to inhibit that behavior. It is roughly analogous to telling a stutterer not to stutter. Anyone familiar with classrooms and programs in autism will recognize the value of that cautionary comment.
Conclusion
When I was growing up, speaking was so frustrating. I could see the words in my brain but when I realized that making my mouth move would get those letters to come alive, they died as soon as they were born. What made me feel angry was to know that I knew exactly what I was to say and my brain was retreating in defeat …
Jamie Burke is a college student who now is able to speak the words he types with two fingers on his Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) device. He requires no physical support for his typing yet continues to need the presence of a trusted support person as an "emotional platform" (Maurer, 1993) to execute his typing. It may be that the relationship allows him to be in a more optimal "space" to regulate the sensory and movement aspects of typing. We have proposed that many other individuals with the autism label may be challenged by sensory and movement differences in starting, stopping, executing, combining and/or switching actions, thoughts, emotions and speech. These symptoms have been described in the literature for many years but generally not integrated into our descriptions or understanding of autistic behaviors.
Sensory and movement differences often escape the notice of those of us who do not typically experience them but have been well described by autistic self-advocates and persons interested in individuals with autism and other disability labels. Ignoring these differences (or redefining them as autistic behaviors to be controlled) has made life unnecessarily more difficult for individuals with autism and those who care about and for them. Many of the assumptive errors we have made are based on our own social history. In the absence of clarity about the nature of these movement differences, we will continue to be forced into the default position of seeing all unfamiliar behaviors as intentional, deliberate evidence of intellectual impairments and even pleasurable. We have not proposed another list of deficits but a greater understanding of the complexity of what we call autistic behaviors and the necessity to rethink our assumptions about them. The task is not going to be easy. Such sensory and movement differences are manifest in autism and many other disorders in strikingly unique, personalized and dynamic ways that test present research (and teaching) strategies that rely heavily on a positivist-reductionist philosophy. Yet, some of the brightest scientific lights of the 20th century reminded us that the best way to approach objectivity in science is to view the phenomenon from as many perspectives as possible (Luria, 1939; Edelman, 1992; Shawlow, 1996, Personal Communication). As Einstein shared, "Not everything that counts can be counted and not everything that's counted, counts" (Cunningham & Scott, 2004).
There is a long, continual path of misunderstanding to autism. People have been thought of, and referred to, as "non-persons," "behavior problems" and sub-normal in every imaginable way. If they cannot speak, we assume they have little to say and offer only the most limited of communication options. Irrespective of the precision and intensity of our interventions, more often than not they experience isolation, segregation, homogeneous grouping, loneliness, pain and boredom as part of their customary care across the life span. Often their sensory and movement differences contribute to such outcomes as these leave the rest of us unaware of the true nature of their challenges.
Any view of autism at this time needs to reflect the experience of self-advocates with autism and others who describe sensory and movement differences, as well as the latest in the neuroscience and child development literature. We need a research agenda that focuses on understanding and supporting autistic people and others in more respectful, personalized and successful ways. It is the least dangerous assumption (Donnellan, 1984) to see all as full human beings who may have formidable and unfamiliar challenges to overcome and who, of course, desire social interaction, communication and participation.
Too often autistic children are raised to believe they are broken and need to be fixed. Adults with autism too often live lives of isolation and poverty. Understanding people's experiences may lead to acceptance, accommodation and appropriate support. To continue down the same paths, well worn for 65 years, when all these data impel us to rethink our assumptions and broaden our path is unthinkable.
Works Cited
- A is For Autism. British Film Institute (1992). "Fine Take Production" for Channel 4.
- Abrams, R, & Taylor, MA. (1976). Catatonia: a prospective clinical study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33(5), 579-581.
- American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: Text revision. (4th ed.) Washington, D.C: Author.
- Baggs, Amanda (2007) I knew moving took effort, but... Retrieved January 15, 2009, from Ballastexistenz. http://ballastexistenz.autistics.org/?p=379
- Baltaxe, C, & Simmons, J. (1977). Bedtime soliloquies and linguistic competence in autism. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 42, 376-393.
- Barron, J., & Barron, S. (1992). There's a boy in here. New York: Simon & Schuster.
- Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. New York: Ballantine.
- Bedrova, E. and Leong, D. (1996). Tools of the mind: The Vygotskian approach to childhood education. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
- Blackman, L. (1999). Lucy's story: Autism and other adventures. Brisbane, Australia: Book in Hand.
- Bluestone, J. (2005). The Fabric of Autism: Weaving the threads into a cogent theory. Seattle, Washington: Sapphire Enterprises.
- Bristol, M.M., Cohen, D.J., Costello, E.J., Denckla, M., Eckberg, T.J., Kallen, R., Kraemer, H.C., Lord, C., Maurer, R., McIlvane, W.J., Minshew, N., Sigman, M. & Spence, M. A. (1996). State of the science in autism: Report to the National Institutes of Health. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 121-154.
- Burke, J. (2005). The world as I'd like it to be. In D. Biklen (Eds.), Autism and the myth of the person alone. New York: NYU Press.
- Caroff, S. & Ungvari, G. (2007). Expanding horizons in catatonic research. Guest Editorial, Psychiatric Annals, 37(1), 7-9.
- Condon, W.S. (1985). Sound-film microanalysis: A means for correlating brain and behavior. In F. Duffy & N. Geschwind (Eds.), Dyslexia: A neuroscientific approach to clinical evaluation. Boston: Little-Brown Co.
- Cunningham, H. and Scott, D. (2004). Software architecture for language engineering. Natural Language Engineering, 10(3/4), 205-209.
- Damasio, A.R. & Maurer, R.G. (1978). A neurological model for childhood autism. Archives of Neurology, 35(12), 777-786.
- Dhossche, D.M. ( 2004). Autism as early expression of catatonia. Medical Science Monitor, 10(3), RA31-9.
- Donnellan, A.M. (1984). The criterion of the least dangerous assumption. Behavior Disorders, 9, 141-150.
- Donnellan, A.M, Leary, M., & Robledo, J. (2006). I can't get started: Stress and the role of movement differences for individuals with the autism label. In M.G. Baron, J. Groden, G. Groden, & L. Lipsitt (Eds.), Stress and coping in autism (pp. 205-245). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Donnellan, A. M., Mirenda, P., Mesaros, R. A., & Fassbender, L. (1984). A strategy for analyzing the communicative functions of behavior. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 2, 201-212.
- Edelman, Gerald M. (1992). Bright air, brilliant fire. New York: Basic Books.
- Endow, Judy (2006). Making lemonade: Hints for autism's helpers. Cambridge, Wisconsin: Cambridge Book Review Press.
- Fay, W. (1969). On the basis of autistic echolalia. Journal of Communicative Disorders, 2, 38-47.
- Feldenkrais, M. (1972). Awareness through movement. New York: HarperCollins.
- Filipek, P., Accardo, P, Ashwal, S., Baranek, G.,Cook, E.,Jr., Dawson, G., Gordon, D., Gravel, J.,Johnson, C., Kallen, R.,Levy, S.,Minshew, N.,Ozonoff,S.,Prizant, B., Rapin, I., Rogers, S., Stone, W.,Teplin, S.,Tuchman, R.,& Volkmar, F.(2000). Practice parameter: Screening and diagnosis of autism. Neurology 55(4), 468-479.
- Fink, M. & Taylor, M.A. (2003). Catatonia: A Clinician's guide to diagnosis and treatment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Fink, M. & Taylor, M.A. (2006). Catatonia: Subtype or syndrome in DSM? Editorial in American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(11), 1875-1876
- Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
- Gernsbacher, M.A., Sauer, E.A., Geye, H.M., Schweigert, E.K. & Goldsmith, H.H. (2008). Infant and toddler oral- and manual- motor skills predict later speech fluency in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 49(1), 43- 50.
- Ghaziuddin, M & Butler, E. (1998). Clumsiness in autism and asperger syndrome: A further report. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 42, 43-48.
- Gibson, J.J. (1979). The ecological approach to perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
- Goldman S, Wang C, Salgado MW, Greene PE, Kim M, Rapin I. (2009). Motor stereotypies in children with autism and other developmental disorders. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 51(1), 30-38.
- Gilles de la Tourette, G. (1885). Etude sur nerveus caracteristee par de l'incoordination motrice accompagnee d'echolalie et de copralie. Archs Neurol., 9, 19-42, 158-200.
- Grandin, T. (1992). An insider view of autism. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.). High functioning individuals with autism (pp.105-124). New York: Springer.
- Hale, M., & Hale, C. (1999). I had no means to shout! Bloomington, IN: 1st Books.
- Green, D., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Simonoff, E. & Baird, G. (2009). Impairment of movement skills of children with autistic spectrum disorders. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 51, 311-316.
- Harp. B. (2008). Square talk: Processing. Asperger Square 8, on-line blog, http://aspergersquare8.blogspot.com/
- Hill, D.A. & Leary, M.R. (1993). Movement disturbance: A clue to hidden competencies in persons diagnosed with autism and other developmental disabilities. Madison, WI: DRI Press.
- Hill, D.A. & Leary, M.R. (in press). Casting call for a supporting role. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability.
- Jacobson, J..W. & Ackerman, L.J. (1990). Differences in adaptive functioning among people with autism or mental retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 205-219.
- Jansiewicz, E., Goldberg, M., Newschaffer, C., Denckla, M., Landa, R., Mostofsky, S. (2006). Motor signs distinguish children with high functioning autism and Asperger's syndrome from controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(5), 613- 621.
- Kahlbaum, K. (1973). Catatonia (Y. Levij & T. Pridan, Trans.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. (Original work published in 1874)
- Kanner, L. (1946). Irrelevant and metaphorical language in early infantile autism. American Journal of Psychiatry,103, 242-246.
- Leary, M. & Hill, D. (1996). Moving on: Autism and movement disturbance. Mental Retardation, 34(1), 39-53.
- Leekam SR, Nieto C, Libby SJ, Wing L, Gould J. (2007). Describing the sensory abnormalities of children and adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(5).
- Lovaas, O.I. (1966). A program for the establishment of speech in psychotic children. In J. Wing (Ed). Childhood autism. London: Pegramon Press.
- Lovaas, O.I. (1977). The autistic child: Language development through behavior modification. New York: Halstead Press.
- Lovaas, O.I., Schreibman,L., & Koegel, R.L (1974). A behavior modification approach to the treatment of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4, 111-129.
- Lovett, H. (1996). Learning to listen: Positive approaches and people with difficult behavior. Baltimore MD: Paul H. Brookes.
- Luria, A. R. (1932). The nature of human conflicts or emotion, conflict and will. New York: Liveright, Inc.
- Magg, J.W., Wolchik, S.A., Rutherford, Jr., R. Parks, B.T. (1986). Response covariation on self-stimulatory behaviors during sensory extinction procedures. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 119-132.
- Maurer, R. G. (1993). What autism and facilitated communication have to teach us about the neurology of relationship (Audiotape from lecture). Toronto: MacKenzie Group International.
- McGinnity, K. & Negri, N. (2005). Walk awhile in my autism. Cambridge, WI: Cambridge Book Review Press.
- McGoon, D. C. (1994). The parkinson's handbook. New York: W.W. Norton.
- Markram, H., Rinaldi, T. & Markram, K. (2007). The intense world syndrome — An alternative hypothesis for autism. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 1(1), 77- 96.
- Mirenda, P., & Donnellan, A. (1986). Effects of adult interaction style on conversational behavior in students with severe communication problems. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 17, 126-141.
- Mostofsky, S.H. (2008). Anomalous Motor Circuit Activity and Connectivity in Autism. Paper presented at the meeting of the Child Neurology Society, Santa Clara, CA.
- Mostofsky, S., Dubey, P., Jerath, V., Jansiewicz, E., Goldberg, M., & Denckla, M. (2006). Developmental dyspraxia is not limited to imitation in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(03), 314-326.
- Mostofsky, S. H., Powell, S. K., Simmonds, D. J., Goldberg, M. C., Caffo, B., & Pekar, J. J. (2009). Decreased connectivity and cerebellar activity in autism during motor task performance. Brain.
- Mulick, J.A., Jacobson, J.W. & Kobe, F.H. (1993). Anguished silence and helping hands: Autism and facilitated communication. Skeptical Inquirer, 17, 270-280.
- Nayate, A., Bradshaw, J., Rinehart, N. (2005). Autism and asperger's disorder: Are they movement disorders involving the cerebellum and/or basal ganglia? Brain Research Bulletin, 67, 327-334.
- Noterdaeme, M., Mildenberger, K., Minow, F. & Amorosa, H. (2002). Evaluation of neuromotor deficits in children with autism and children with a specific speech and language disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 11(5).
- Penland, H.R., Weder, N. & Tampi, R.R. (2006). The catatonic dilemma expanded. Annals of General Psychiatry, 5(14), 1-9.
- Prizant, B. & Duchan, J. (1981). The functions of immediate echolalia in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46, 241-249.
- Prizant, B. & Rydell, P. (1984). Analysis of functions of delayed echolalia in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 183-192.
- Rimland, B. (1993). Editor's notebook. Autism Research Review International, 7(3), 3.
- Rincover, A. (1978). Sensory extinction: A procedure for eliminating self-stimulatory behavior in developmentally disabled children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 6(3), 299-310.
- Rinehart, N., Tonge, B., Iansek, R., McGinley, J., Brereton, A., Enticott, P., et al. (2006). Gait function in newly diagnosed children with autism: Cerebellar and basal ganglia related motor disorder. Developmental medicine and child neurology, 48(10), 819-24.
- Robledo, J & Donnellan, A. (2008). Properties of supportive relationships from the perspective of academically successful individuals with autism. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 46(4), 299-310.
- Rogers, D. (1992). Motor disorder in psychiatry: Towards a neurological psychiatry. Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons.
- Rogers, S. & Benetto, L. (2002). Le fonctionnement moteur dans le cas d'autisme. Enfance, 54(1), 63-73.
- Rubin, S., Biklen, D., Kasa-Hendrickson, C., Kluth, P., Cardinal, D. & Broderick, A. (2001). Independence, participation, and the meaning of intellectual ability. Disability & Society, 16(3), 415-429.
- Sacks, O.W. (1989). Neuropsychiatry and tourette's. In Mueller, J. (Ed.) Neurology and psychiatry: A meeting of minds. Basel: Karger.
- Sacks, O. (1990). Awakenings. New York: Harper Perennial.
- Savarese, R. (2007). Reasonable People: Autism and Adoption. New York: Other Press.
- Schwarz, P. (2004). Building alliances: Community identity and the role of allies in autistic self-advocacy. In Stephen Shore (Ed.) Ask and tell: Self-advocacy and disclosure for people on the autisms spectrum. Shawnee Mission, KS: Autism Asperger Publishing Co.
- Starkstein, S. E., Goldar, J. C., & Hodgkiss, A. (1995). Karl Ludwig Kahlbaum's concept of catatonia. History of Psychiatry, 6(22 Pt 2), 201-7.
- Strandt-Conroy, K. (1999). Exploring movement differences in autism through firsthand accounts. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin, Madison.
- Stern, D.N. (2000). The Interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.
- Sigman, M., & Spence, M.A. (1996). State of the science in autism: Report to the National Institutes of Health. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(2), 121-154.
- Sullivan, A. (2002). Inertia: From theory to praxis. Retrieved from http://www.autistics.org/library/inertia.html
- Sutera, S., Pandey, J., Esser, E., Rosenthal, M., Wilson, L., Barton, M. Green, J., Hodgson., Robins, D., Dumont-Mathieu, T., & Fein, D. (2007). Predictors of optimal outcome in toddlers diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 98-107.
- Taylor, M. & Fink, M. (2003). Catatonia in psychiatric classification: A home of its own. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1233-1241.
- Taylor, B.A., Hoch, H, Weissman, M. (2005). The analysis and treatment of vocal stereotypy in a child with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 20(4), 239-253.
- Thelen, E. (1995). Motor development: A new synthesis. American Psychologist, 50(2), 79-95.
- Thelen, E. & Smith, L.B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Tomchek, S. D., & Dunn, W. (2007). Sensory processing in children with and without autism: A comparative study using the short sensory profile. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy: Official Publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association, 61(2), 190-200.
- Williams, D. (1992). Nobody nowhere. New York: Avon.
- Williams, D. (1994). Somebody somewhere. New York: Times Books.
- Williams, D. (1996a). Like color to the blind. New York: Times Books.
- Williams, D. (1996b). Autism: An inside-out approach. London: Jessica Kingsley.
- Williams, D. (2003). Exposure anxiety — The invisible cage: An exploration of self-protection responses in the autism spectrum disorders. London: Jessica Kingsley.
- Wing, L. (1981). Language, social and cognitive impairments in autism and severe mental retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 31-44.
- Wing, L. & Attwood, A. (1987). Syndromes of autism and atypical development. In D. Cohen & A. Donnellan (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 3-19). New York: Wiley.
- Ylvisker, M. & Feeney, T. J. (1998). Collaborative brain injury intervention: Positive everyday routines. San Diego: Singular Publishing Group, Inc.