Abstract

After more than a decade with no quantitative update on the progress of advertising's integration of disabled characters this study found 29 out of 1,671 prime-time commercials analyzed included images of disability. Although the raw number is only 1.7% of the total sample it represents a 200% increase over 1999 totals. The portrayals were much more likely to be major characters than background roles and the disability depicted in the commercials tended to be from a more diverse set of conditions than it was in the late 1980s. Based on an approximately equal sample of television programming the ability-integrated advertising (AI advertising) appeared more frequently on cable channels (52%) than network channels (39%).

In 2001 Disability Studies Quarterly dedicated an entire issue to the topic of disability and advertising. As part of that issue Ganahl and Arbuckle presented a content analysis of prime time commercials that became the most often cited work in disability literature when referring to quantitative estimates of consumer advertising that contained images of the disable. From the entire pool of almost 3,000 television commercials coded, they found only 15 (.5%) contained character roles that met the coding parameters of physical disability for the study (Ganahl & Arbuckle, 2001). At the time even the very small frequency of these roles was considered a positive sign of recognition of the disability community by advertisers. Prior to 1988 the physical imperfections of the disabled made them invisible to an industry that employed a minimum standard for physical appearance (Hahn, 1987).

Since that issue was published studies examining the portrayal of disability in advertising have adopted qualitative content analysis as the preferred methodology. As justification for analyzing these commercial messages at least one respected researcher cited the Ganahl and Arbuckle study as proof of the "…inefficiency of quantitative methods" (Haller, 2010, p.194). It is hard to argue against qualitative content analysis when levels of occurrence are less than 1% as they were in the Ganahl and Arbuckle study. The qualitative approach also has provided significant evidence of positive thematic change and expansion of the types of disability portrayed in advertising messages in the U.S. and the U. K. (Haller & Ralph, 2006; Haller & Ralph, 2001).

But more than 20 years after McDonald's included a scene depicting students signing about going to the burger chain in their television commercial and Target included a model in a wheelchair in it's flyer, isn't it time to see if this particular idea is being adopted by the advertising industry? While the most recent examinations of media depictions of disability give us a guide to how people with disabilities are being portrayed, in terms of societal adoption or even impact of a practice such as the industry's use of disabled character roles in advertising, it is important to place context on the practice. It is fair to ask the question of whether the portrayal of disability in consumer advertising has reached a significant level with regard to frequency in the actual media environment so that the public sees the actor and not the disability. In essence there is value to asking the question, "Are we there yet?"

For this study a quantitative content analysis of disability portrayals in prime time television commercials was conducted. The study provided a much-needed update to the literature and also examined whether events of the past decade have influenced the diffusion of AI advertising within the industry.

Background

The media's contribution to the push for social integration of the disabled became the focus of several communication researchers in the 1990s because one of the positive outcomes from the battle for passage of the ADA was an era of heightened visibility and awareness of disability issues on the part of the media. In the 10 years that followed passage of the ADA, the disability community began to appear in front of the camera in ways that were a departure from the pattern of negative stereotypes that painted the disabled as either evil or a burden on society (Black & Pretes, 2007; Byrd, 1989; Safran, 1998). The previously "invisible minority" (Nelson, 2003 p. 175) enjoyed positive portrayals, albeit in small amounts, in entertainment media, traditional news outlets and even advertising.

If one accepts the power of the media to speed diffusion of an idea through a society, place an item on the public agenda, or serve as a learning tool for social behavior, then the increased visibility of people with disabilities in the media since 1990 should have contributed to significant improvements in the area of social acceptance for the disabled. Unfortunately the latest survey of Americans with disabilities conducted by Louis Harris on behalf of the National Organization on Disability (NOD) reported large gaps between disabled and nondisabled respondents' measures of quality of life (www.2010disabilitysurveys.org). It would be naïve to think that just because the media started portraying people with disabilities in a positive light that all barriers to equality might disappear, but "…while movies entertain, they simultaneously provide viewers with information about disabilities…and representations of how individuals fit into a nation's social and political landscape" (Safran, 2001, p. 223). If the images from films, TV and even advertising portray people with disabilities in negative fashion as objects of pity, then society will see them from that perspective.

Classroom SADP Suggests Additional Concerns

Prior to the decision to go forward with this study, we attempted to get some indication of any trend with regard to attitude toward people with disabilities on the part of the nondisabled by administering the Survey of Attitudes toward Disabled People (SADP) scale to 345 college students across the country as part of a classroom introduction to consumer research. The 24-item summative-rating SADP scale required respondents to rate each of 24 statements about disability on a six-point scale ranging from '-3' (I disagree very much) to '+3' (I agree very much). The range of scores was 0 — 144 with a higher score indicating a more favorable attitude toward people with disabilities. The grand mean for the sample was 100.45. To put that into perspective the mean SADP score during original reliability testing by Antonak was 111 (Antonak & Livneh, 1988). A published study on AI advertising in 1996 reported a mean SADP score of 108 for college students in the southeast (Farnall, 1996). Using a one sample t test with Antonak's original number as the population mean, the 2008 measure of 100 is significantly lower (p < .001) indicating a somewhat counter-intuitive trend between expected media exposure and attitude shift. It is important to note that no measure of media exposure to AI advertising was taken for the 2008 sample therefore no direct correlation can be made.

Is our nation's attitude toward people with disabilities becoming more negative as the classroom SADP experience would seem to indicate? Again the role of the media comes into question. In the pre-ADA period several major studies found the depiction of people with disabilities to be negative, unrealistic and stereotypical (Donaldson, 1981; Longmore, 1985b; Zola, 1985). By the early 1990s studies were reporting improvement in frequency and type of portrayal for the disabled (Byrd 1989; Nelson 2003). According to Farnall and Smith (1999), individuals who viewed positive portrayals of people with disabilities on television shows and in films were less likely to report negative emotions associated with disability images. But just like the 1930 Payne studies on film, Gerbner's Cultural Indicators Project, and other well known research that linked societal attitude to media, step one in addressing the media portrayal-attitude question seems to be content analysis.

Baseline AI Advertising Study

As previously mentioned, the only published quantitative content analysis addressing the frequency and portrayal of people with disabilities in television advertising was published in a special issue of Disability Studies Quarterly in 2001. In that study Ganahl and Arbuckle (2001) collected 2,999 prime time network television commercials that aired during two successive February sweeps periods in 1998 and 1999. Each spot was coded for presence of characters displaying physical impairment. In this study a physical impairment was defined as a visible and non-correctable impairment. If those images were found, coders determined whether the character played a primary or secondary role in the commercial. From the entire pool of commercials they found only 15 (.5%) commercials that met the first condition. Although the total number of AI commercials was less than 1% of the total sample, the study provided at least some kind of estimate for disability advertising. If there was a positive sign from this accounting it was that almost 75% of the roles classified as "physically disabled" were also coded as major character roles (Ganahl & Arbuckle, 2001).

Advertiser Concerns Since Baseline

In the almost 14 years since data were collected for that study the advertising landscape has changed drastically but no new quantitative measure of AI advertising has been published. There are reasons to expect a positive growth for AI advertising, including a 2006 quote in powerhouse trade publication Advertising Age that called the connection between people with disabilities and major corporations, "just good business sense" (Voight, 2006); but recent history has called the value of the association into question.

Negative Indicators

There were three AI advertising-related events that may have negatively impacted the diffusion rate for AI advertising in the U.S., and reduced the number of advertisers willing to incorporate disability roles into their marketing strategies. The most visible was the infamous Christopher Reeves commercial. The second was the cancellation of the only national awards program that recognized companies that created positive roles for people with disabilities in advertising. The third was the series of lawsuits that targeted companies affiliated with the disability cause.

  1. Superman bombs. Recent history has shown that writing ads that incorporate disabled characters in general consumer products campaigns can offend more than endear (Cheng, 2002; Sewell, 2005). The most famous example was the Christopher Reeves spot of 2000 for an investment company in which Reeves was portrayed as walking again thanks to funded research. Critics of the ad said that Reeves' refusal to accept his quadriplegia was offensive to those who have learned to live with their disabilities (Nelson, 2000). In addition the computer graphics used to import Reeves' head on the body of an actor resulted in a less than proportionate figure. In particular, people with spinal cord injuries protested that the Reeves' spot damaged marketing efforts on spinal cord research and presented a false picture that mobility was only a matter of time (Sewell, 2004). Reeves actually defended the spot in an interview with Diane Sawyer saying that he thought the quality of the production and the simplicity of the message made it an appropriate message.
  2. Advertising agencies loose a chance to shine. Success in advertising has been operationalized in many ways, but the number of awards won has been one standard used by agencies and advertisers alike for decades (Till & Baack, 2005). In fact one estimate placed the number of advertising award shows at more than 500 worldwide (Maxwell & Wanta, 1998). It has been 10 years since Easter Seals cancelled the biggest and longest running AI advertising awards program in the country, the nationally recognized Equality, Dignity and Independence (EDI) awards program. Since 1989 the EDI Awards program had attempted to recognize superior broadcast and print advertising that included positive portrayals of people with disabilities. The EDI program brought advertising agencies and their clients national publicity for "doing the right thing." The show was a major media event in New York, costing several hundred thousand dollars and often featuring major stars as guest hosts. It proved to be a popular event with the industry as entries in the third year of the competition had increased 300% (Farnall, 2000). While it may not be a politically correct statement, the truth is that advertisers and agencies like to win awards and they benefit from the press they receive, especially when the brand is associated with positive community action. Cancellation of this program may have had a negative impact on advertisers' motivation to design and execute AI advertising.
  3. ADA lawsuits plague corporations. The volley of AI ads in the mid 90s was followed by a wave of ADA-related lawsuits. American Online, Wal-Mart and Kmart paid out approximately $25 million in ADA related claims (Voight, 2006). These lawsuits made marketers cautious about associating with the disability issue at all and cut down on the number of clients asking to be associated with people with disabilities in any communication (Voight, 2006). No figures were available for total claims paid but it is safe to say that companies do not like paying settlements on lawsuits.

Research Questions

Clearly there are mixed messages in terms of the history of AI advertising and no real body of evidence that suggests the amount has increased or decreased. One would assume that with the success of major advertisers like Target, McDonalds, Levis, Toys "R" Us, and Cingular Wireless, more and more companies would expand their focus to include people with disabilities. Some marketers have even changed company policy to make sure minorities and people with disabilities are included (Voight, 2006). But issues such as those presented here make the answer to the question of representation unclear leading us to the following questions:

RQ#1a. Has the amount of AI advertising in prime time television programming on both network and cable channels increased from the levels measured one decade ago?
RQ#1b. Is there a difference in the amount of current AI advertising aired on cable versus network television?
RQ#2. Has the composition of AI ADVERTISING portrayals varied since the first Ganahl and Arbuckle (2001) study?

Method

The first content analysis of AI advertising commercials by Ganahl and Arbuckle (2001) provided a good starting point for determining an appropriate sample size and unit of analysis in this study. However, because cable viewership has grown substantially from when the data was collected in 1998 and 1999, this analysis was expanded to include cable networks. It was also determined that cable channels might be more heavily used vehicles for AI advertising because smaller cable audiences mean lower advertising rates and cable provides some narrowly niched programming that would be of interest to this market.

Sampling Procedure

The sampling frame for this content analysis was defined as all channels broadcasting prime-time programming on the local cable system. In this case, the system offered 96 channels to subscribers getting "expanded" basic service. The expanded basic service was the most popular choice according to the communication office of that cable system. These 96 different channel offerings included all the major networks, four local independent stations, 4 local access/public broadcast stations and a number of Spanish language channels. After reviewing the list of channels, the Spanish language channels were removed from the pool because the researchers feared coding problems due to translation issues. Public access channels were removed from the pool because of the unique nature of advertising on these channels. The remaining stations were then subjected to a stratified sample selection process as described in Krippendorff (1980). Network channels were placed in one stratum, independents and all other channels in the second strata. Because the Ganahl and Arbuckle study looked at networks only and a direct comparison of 1998-99 network against 2009 network AI advertising was desired, all four major networks were included. The final channels to make up the rest of the sample were then randomly selected from each stratum. The final sample pool of 10 channels included the four networks, Discovery, CW, Lifetime, A&E, ESPN and one independent.

In the Ganahl and Arbuckle (2001) study, only the major networks were included in the sample and the content was collected during two February sweeps periods (Sunday through Saturday) by recording programming for the entire three-hour prime-time period each day. While the authors did not note that approach as a limitation of their study, focusing on only network programming and concentrating the time span of the collection to only one week (sweeps week at that) seems to present an incomplete picture.

For this study, investigators viewed 50 hours of programming recorded over the course of 21 days in 2009. Each of the 10 channels that made up the sample was recorded for three to five hours in one-hour blocks. The one-hour blocks were randomly determined by drawing from three slips of paper with the prime-time blocks (7:00-7:59 p.m., 8:00—8:59 p.m., and 9:00-9:59 p.m. CST) recorded on them. Then for each time block drawn the day of the week was drawn from another container. After each drawing, all slips of paper were returned to the pool for the next drawing.

Unit of Analysis

As in the first study (Ganahl & Arbuckle, 2001) the unit of analysis was each individual commercial announcement. Coders were instructed to include all ads that were more than 10 seconds in length. The 10-second rule was adopted for this study to discount quick programming notes that were often included in the commercial break. With the exception of that 10-second rule the type of commercial made no difference (local, national, station promo or PSA).

Procedure

Investigators recruited coders from an undergraduate research methods class at a major university. Those students who were interested in receiving the extra credit offered for the task were instructed to attend a training session held outside class. At the training session investigators briefed the students on the project including a discussion of disability, a review of the codebook and instructions and a demonstration of the coding process using several sample AI advertising. All the students who attended the coding sessions decided to continue with the project and were given a set of coding sheets and an instruction sheet that included the channel/time/day to be recorded. So that inter-coder reliability could be calculated, investigators had 20% of the 50 one-hour blocks coded by two different people and their results were compared.

Coding variables

For each commercial, coders were required to make only three decisions. First they were to decide whether any characters in each ad displayed a physical or mental disability. Surprisingly that decision was not as free of conflict as the authors expected. The initial Scott's Pi measure for this variable was 0.88, but with some clarification on whether characters in prescription medicine advertising should be counted, the percent of agreement on the first variable topped 0.95. The specific issue that caused the most disagreement was advertising for medications used to treat depression, anxiety and even erectile dysfunction. For the purposes of this study those illnesses were not included. Support for the decision to eliminate these commercials comes from the method description in the Ganahl and Arbuckle (2001) study. From that article it is clear such illnesses were not coded as including people with disabilities. For that study a physical impairment was defined as "a visible and non-correctable impairment that is of a physical, sensory, or developmental nature" (2001 p. 4).

The second decision required coders to classify the commercial as being for a local establishment or one that is regional/national. The coding instruction book made it clear that any chain business that had locations in other states was to be considered national. As a result the Scott's Pi for this variable was .98.

The final coding decision required the coder to decide whether the role was a major or background role. Major characters were defined as actors who were normally on the screen for at least half the total commercial time. Background roles were defined as those in which the actor appeared for only a few seconds. That operationalization for background and major character roles was in line with the Ganahl and Arbuckle study. An inter-coder reliability level of 98% (Scott's Pi = .982) was achieved for this variable.

Results

Fifty hours of network and cable television programming were analyzed. The programming contained a total of 1,692 commercial messages. Of those, 21 did not meet the required length standard and were discarded even though they were included by a coder. The final sample contained a total of 1,671 valid commercials that were content analyzed.

 VideoAI ADVERTISING
TypeHours of ProgrammingPercent of TotalNumber of SpotsPercent of Total
Network2448%1139%
Cable2346%1552%
Independent36%310%

Table 1 compares the number of times a disabled character appeared in a commercial based on type of channel. With approximately equal hours of programming, cable channels (52%) carried more AI advertising than network channels (39%). Local independent channels accounted for 10% of the AI ads, but represented only 6% of the total hours programmed. The results are not counter-intuitive but do present an additional area of investigation in the future, namely whether local or spot advertising across channels carries a disproportionate amount of AI advertising.

RQ#1a: Is there more AI advertising now than in the previous study?

In the final sample, only 29 commercials (1.7%) contained characters that displayed a disability. Six of the commercials were local, six were channel promotions and 17 were for nationally distributed products. The type of disability portrayed in those 29 commercials (based on the categories of interest) included 17% wheelchair user, 17% blind, 17% mental disability, 10% amputee, 4% deaf and 35% in the "other" category. While the overall percentage of television commercials including images of disability in this sample is still small, it is considerably larger than the total reported in Ganahl and Arbuckle (2001). The results of a chi square test suggest that the current study's totals differ significantly on number of AI ads from the 2001 study's totals (Χ2(1, N = 4670) = 9.72, p < .01). When examining the percentages of AI advertisements, it is clear the number of ads increased in the last decade (current = 1.7%, previous = 0.5%). Based on this sample expressed as a percentage of all advertising, the answer to the first research question is the amount of AI advertising has increased more than 200%.

RQ#1b. Difference in AI advertising between cable and network programming.

RQ#1 was phrased in a manner that addressed the amount of AI advertising for both network and cable channels. Cable channels did deliver more AI commercials (18 commercials) than network (11 commercials), however, data analysis suggests there is no significant difference between the number of AI advertisements on cable and network (Χ2(1, n = 1671) = 1.837, p > .10).

RQ#2: Character Portrayal

RQ#2 addresses the issue of type of portrayal of disabled characters in either major or minor roles. An initial review of the data revealed the expected count for minor roles in the 2001 study was less than five, therefore a chi square analysis was not run, however frequencies were compared.

In the 2001 study, 13 of 18 portrayals, or 72.2%, were coded as primary roles (appearing on the screen for a majority of the time and directly interacting with the product). In the current study, the percentage of major roles based on total AI advertising portrayals was 68.9%, 20 of 29 portrayals. While the number of portrayals increased dramatically the type of role remained relatively constant. Since a majority of the roles for disabled actors were major roles, even in the 2001 study, the fact that the percentage has remained steady must be viewed as positive.

Discussion

Barbara Kolucki, a consultant on disability and children's media, writing for Rehabilitation International, posed the question, "Has our public education and media efforts made a difference?" (2009). In answering her own question Kolucki points out that there are simply too many variables in the world of attitude formation and change to be able to isolate the contribution of any one. For example, is it advocacy, legislation, public education or media that caused change? Her basic approach to the question was that it does not matter as long as change occurs.

The 200% increase in the amount of prime time TV AI advertising over levels measured in 1998-99 could be interrupted as proof of change on the part of the advertising industry. Haller and Ralph (2006; 2001) established that advertising storylines/themes had changed with their qualitative focus on AI advertising and now this quantitative study confirms that the advertising community is adopting the practice of including images of disability in its executions. The increase in the amount of AI advertising from this content analysis over the 1999 level is a sign that efforts from media and groups like the American Association of People with Disabilities have made a positive contribution to representation of the disability community in advertising.

The 1.7% total for AI advertising in this study also compares favorably with the two most recent content analysis studies that looked at percentage of disabled character portrayal in the entertainment industry. A study of role portrayal in prime time Japanese TV found an identical 1.7 % of roles displaying disability during prime time (Saito & Ishiyama, 2005). Henderson and Heinz-Knowles' (2003) content analysis for prime time U.S. TV during Fall 2000 and Fall 2001 found only .6% of the roles were for disabled actors. Interestingly data for their study was collected about the same time as the baseline AI study by Ganahl (2001) and the total representation for entertainment media was about the same as advertising. If a more recent study of prime time entertainment programming were available for the U.S. it would allow for an interesting examination of trends in disability portrayal amounts across genres.

In terms of the type of portrayal, this study reported a 2.6% drop in the percentage of major roles dedicated to disabled characters. Even with the drop there are still twice as many major roles for people with disabilities as there are minor roles. Considering that many of the early efforts at integration as demonstrated by television commercials entered in the Equality, Dignity and Independence (EDI) competition in the 1980s made use of the "background kid in a wheelchair" approach, one would have to view the findings of this study in a positive light. Also, the fact that the portrayal of disabled individuals took such varied form in this sample should be viewed as a signal that organizations involved in casting and producing AI advertising for the past 20 years are having a positive impact. These casting organizations such as the Media Access Office, Solution Marketing Group, and WC Duke Associates have been successfully guiding clients through the pitfalls of AI advertising execution for more than a decade (Krossel, 2001).

As Media Exposure Increases…

In the last 10 years many would argue that the media, including advertisers, have done much to project a more positive image of people with disabilities. The correlation between exposure, through personal contact or media portrayal, and positive attitude has been made in the disability literature over and over again (Farnall & Smith, 1999; Nelson, 2000; Shapiro, 1993; Yuker, 1988). Several notable disability advocates have echoed the familiarity theme as a major influence on attitude including Dr. I. King Jordon, Gallaudet University president who said, "The most important barriers to the employment of disabled people are attitudinal barriers. We are often judged unable to do something before we're given an opportunity to try. The worst thing about having a disability is that people meet it before they meet you" (Hopkins & Nestleroth, 1991, p. 74).

So why then did the SADP measure for college students in 2009 show a significant drop over previous studies? The increase in amount and type of AI advertising over the past decade reported in this study should signal a positive shift in the measure of attitudes toward people with disabilities, not the significant decline reported early in this paper. It is this counter-intuitive finding that leads this author to suggest there are two areas of future research that should follow this study. The first is a very complete content analysis of characters in prime-time television broadcasting in the US. The Media Access Office (MAO) has been tracking roles received by their clients since 2000 but many individuals who are SAG members would not be included in the MAO's count. For example Hugh Laurie plays a physically disabled doctor in Fox's House, but he is not a member of the MAO nor does his character get included in their count. Although a large undertaking getting some kind of quantitative count would help paint a clearer picture of what America is watching.

Richard Antonak, the creator of the SADP scale, suggested the other vein of research that should be a follow-up to these content analyses. In an article written for Disability and Rehabilitation Antonak and Livneh (2000) suggested that to obviate validity threats researchers should consider both indirect measures and physiological methods. In particular he suggested that electromyography might provide accurate measures yet no applications of this specific technology for measurement of attitudes could be found. A number of studies have attempted to develop multidimensional attitude scales and implicit association tests as a better means of obtaining true measures of attitude toward the disabled but these attempts have not been widely accepted (Findler, Vilchinsky, & Werner, 2007; Pruett & Chan, 2006; Thomas, Vaughn, & Doyle, 2007). If a reliable and valid measure were adopted that indicated attitude toward the disabled had become more negative in the face of more and more positive media portrayals it would suggest more research is needed in the area of attitude formation and change with regard to the disabled.

Are we There Yet?

A great many government organizations, charities, advocacy groups, consulting firms and media organizations are expending enormous amounts of resources because they all agree with the premise that television is a powerful educator for cultural values and attitudes. Social learning theory, cultivation and media dependency theory all support that premise, as does a body of work in the rehabilitation literature (Elliott & Byrd, 1983; Hopkins & Nestleroth, 1991). In the small world of AI advertising the results of this study suggest that images of people with disabilities in advertising are a bigger part of the overall advertising environment than they were in 2001 despite the difficulties associated with contextual elements in an ad, and the public outrage/civil suits. But if we compare the appearance rate for AI advertising, as part of the total advertising environment, to the percentage of adults classified as disabled in the total U.S. population based, people with disabilities are very much under-represented (1.7% from this study as compared to 12% according to the 2009 Disability Compendium). If equal representation is the goal, we are not there yet. If acceptance of the use of disabled portrayals in general product advertising on the part of the advertising industry is the goal, then much progress has been made.

Perhaps a better question to ask at this point is, "How do we get there?" Partnerships between disability advocates like the American Association of People with Disabilities and corporate giants such as American Airlines (Altitude Awards) and Wal-Mart (Internship funding) should certainly continue. But perhaps it is time to target advertising industry professionals. Instead of measuring public perception and response to AI advertising, it may be time to address agency personnel who are responsible for creating and executing advertising strategy. Is AI advertising even an option in their minds? Are ad professionals more positive or negative in their attitudes about AI advertising? For those agencies that have used AI advertising in the past, was the motivation client-driven as was the case with much of the early AI advertising (Sewall, 2005)? Answers to these questions may shed light on the acceptance of AI advertising and give advocates a clearer picture of just how long it might be before seeing a person with a disability pitch breakfast sausage or shop for family gifts at the local K Mart is just good advertising instead of advocacy advertising.

Works Cited

  • Antonak, R. F., & Livneh, H. (1988). The measurement of attitudes toward people with disabilities. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
  • Antonak, R. F., & Livneh, H. (2000). Measurement of attitudes towards persons with disabilities. Disability and Rehabilitation, 22(5), 211-224.
  • Black, R. S., & Pretes, L. (2007). Victims and victors: Representation of physical disability on the silver screen. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 32(1), 66-83.
  • Byrd, E. K. (1989a). A study of depiction of specific characteristics of characters with disability in film. Journal of Applied Rehabilitation Counseling, 20(2), 43-45.
  • Byrd, E. K. (1989b). Theory regarding attitudes and how they may relate to media portrayals of disability. Journal of Applied Rehabilitation Counseling, 20(4), 36-38.
  • Cheng, K. (2002). What marketers should know about people with disabilities. Retrieved April 25, 2004, from http://www.nod.org/.
  • Donaldson, J. (1980). Changing attitudes toward handicapped persons: A review and analysis of research. Exceptional Children, 46, 504-514.
  • Donaldson, J. (1981). The visibility and image of handicapped people on television. Exceptional Children, 47, 413-416.
  • Elliott, S. (2009, March 30). Marketers lend voice to show support for the disabled. New York Times. Retrieved February 1, 2010, from www.nytimes.com.
  • Elliott, T., & Byrd, E. K. (1983). Attitude change toward disability through television portrayal. Journal of Rehabilitation Counseling, 14. 35-37.
  • England-Kennedy, E. (2008). Media representations of attention deficit disorder: Portrayals of cultural skepticism in popular media. The Journal of Popular Culture, 41(1), 91-117.
  • Farnall, O. F. (1996). Invisible no more: Advertising and people with disabilities. In D. Braithwaite & T. Thompson (Eds.), Handbook of Communication and People with Disabilities: Research and Application (pp. 307-318). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum & Associates.
  • Farnall, O. F. (2000). Positive images of the disabled in television advertising: Effects of marketing measures. In G. Wilcox (Ed.), Proceedings of the 1996 Conference of the American Academy of Advertising (pp.123-130). Austin: American Academy of Advertising.
  • Farnall, O. F., & Smith, K. A. (1999). Reaction to people with disabilities: Personal contact versus viewing of specific media portrayals. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 76, 659-672.
  • Findler, L., Vilchinsky, N., & Werner, S. (2007). The multidimensional attitudes scale toward persons with disabilities (MAS): Construction and validation. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 50(3). 166-176.
  • Ganahl, D. G., & Arbuckle, M. (2001). The exclusion of persons with physical disabilities from prime time television advertising: A two year quantitative analysis. Disability Studies Quarterly, 21(2). Retrieved November 15, 2008, from http://www.dsq-sds.org/.
  • Haller, B. A., Dorries, B., & Rahn, J. (2006). Media labeling versus the US disability community identity: A study of shifting cultural language. Disability and Society, 21(1), 61-75.
  • Haller, B. A., & Ralph, S. (2006). Are disability images in advertising becoming bold and daring? An analysis of prominent themes in US and UK campaigns. Disability Studies Quarterly, 26(3), Retrieved February 6, 2011, from http://www.dsq-sds.org/.
  • Haller, B. A., & Ralph, S. (2001). Profitability, diversity, and disability images in advertising in the United States and Great Britain. Disability Studies Quarterly, 21(2). Retrieved April 22, 2004, from http://www.dsq-sds.org/.
  • Hahn, H. (1987). Advertising the acceptably employable image: Disability and capitalism. Policy Studies Journal, 15(3). 551-570.
  • Henderson, J. J., & Heinz-Knowles, K. (2003, July). The trend toward hyper-marginalization: Images of disability on prime time television. Paper presented at the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication Annual Meeting, Kansas City, MO.
  • Hopkins, K., & Nestleroth, S. (1991, October 28). Willing to act: The 1991 Louis Harris and Associates Survey. BusinessWeek, p. 35.
  • Kolucki, B. (2009). A Review of Research about Media and Disability: Does it Make a Difference? Retrieved September 22, 2009, from http://www.riglobal.org/publications/media_report/kolucki.html.
  • Krippendorff, K. (1980). Content Analysis: An Introduction to its Methodology. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
  • Krossel, M. (2001, November 20). Selling to the disabled can mean more than ads. New York Times. Retrieved June 9, 2008, from http://www.nytimes.com.
  • Leonard, B. D. (1978). Impaired view: Television portrayal of handicapped people. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston University.
  • Longmore, P. L. (1985a). A note on language and the social identity of disabled people. American Behavioral Scientist, 28, 419-423.
  • Longmore, P. L. (1985b). Screening stereotypes: Images of disabled people. Social Policy, 16, 31-37.
  • Maxwell, A., & Wanta, W. (1998). Planning for success: A look at the relationship between account planning and awards for creativity. In D. Muehling (Ed.), Proceedings of the 1998 Conference of the American Academy of Advertising (pp. 250-256). Pullman: Washington State University.
  • Nelson, J. A. (2000). Media role in building the disability community. Journal of Mass Media Ethics, 15(3), 180-193.
  • Nelson, J. A. (2003). The invisible cultural group: Images of disability. In P. M. Lester & S. D. Ross (Eds.), Images That Injure: Pictorial Stereotypes in the Media (pp. 175-194), Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.
  • PR Newswire. (2009). American Airlines and American Association of People with Disabilities Launch Inaugural "Altitude Award" Call for Entries [press release]. Retrieved September 22, 2009, from http://www.aapd.com/site/apps/nlnet/content3.aspx?c=pvI1IkNWJqE&b=5657869&ct=7673689&notoc=1.
  • Pruett, S. R., & Chan, F. (2006). The development and psychometric validation of the disability attitude implicit association test. Rehabilitation Psychology, 51(3). 202-213.
  • Safran, S. P. (1998). Disability portrayal in film: Reflecting the past, directing the future. Exceptional Children, 64, 227-238.
  • Saito, S., & Ishiyama, R. (2005). The invisible minority: Under-representation of people with disabilities in prime-time TV dramas in Japan," Disability & Society, 20, 437-451.
  • Sewell, E. H. (2005). And here's the pitch: How advertising uses disability. In C. R. Riley (Ed.), Disability and the Media: Prescriptions for Change (pp. 109-129), Hanover, NH: University Press of New England.
  • Shapiro, J. P. (2003). No Pity: People with Disabilities Forging a New Civil Rights Movement. New York: Times Books.
  • Thomas, A., Vaughn, D., & Doyle, A. (2007). Implementation of a computer based implicit association test as a measure of attitudes toward individuals with disabilities. Journal of Rehabilitation, 73(2), 3-14.
  • Till, B. D., & Baack, D. W. (2005). Recall and persuasion: Does creative advertising matter. Journal of Advertising, 34(3), 47-57.
  • Voight, J. (2006, March, 27). Accessibility of disability. Adweek. Retrieved September 22, 2009,from http://www.adweek.com/aw/esearch/article_display.jsp?vnu_content_id=100223369.
  • Yuker, H. E. (1988). Attitudes toward persons with disabilities. New York: Springer Publishing Co.
Return to Top of Page