Abstract

Historically, research on families of children with disabilities has been framed by the medical model, which views disability as a biological deficit, necessarily associated with burden and grief. This body of research does not acknowledge the sociocultural contexts in which the meaning of disability is embedded, or the manner in which deficit-based hegemonic discourses shape the familial experience of having a child with a disability. In this paper, we problematize the medicalization of the phenomenon of disability and resulting research inquiries. We propose the use of narrative inquiry, theoretically grounded in sociocultural perspectives, as a critical research methodology for situating the lives of these families in cultural and sociopolitical contexts. Narrative inquiries complicate conclusions drawn from the medical model and contribute to new insights on the ways in which interpretations of having a child with a disability are mutually negotiated between individuals and society.

Introduction

Families of children with disabilities have long held the scrutiny of researchers and professionals. Dominant discourse on families of children with disabilities, based on assumptions of grief, tragedy, and unmitigated hardship, are strongly influenced by the medical model, which constructs disability as a deficit and as predominantly burdensome for both individuals and families. Historically, this deficit-based construction of disability has informed the questions and methodologies in research concerning these families. Rather than examining whether parents of children with disabilities experience significant stress or chronic grief, medical-model based studies begin with assumptions of pathology and seek to evaluate the extent of psychological distress. Additionally in research based in medical model perspectives, the source of the psychological distress experienced by parents is located in their children's impairments. Conspicuously absent from these studies is reflection on the sociocultural constructions of disability, and the contexts in which the experiences of families are situated. These inquiries also fail to consider how pervasive, deficit-based hegemonic discourses on disability contribute to familial stress and other negative emotions. The perspectives of families of children with disabilities are not represented in this research, as familial experiences are reduced to statistical analyses of negative outcomes.

In this essay, we trace the methodological history of studying families of children with disabilities and situate traditional research in this area in the context of underlying cultural beliefs about disability. Our argument is presented systematically in three sections. In the first, we outline the historical medicalization of the phenomenon of disability and discuss dominant cultural narratives about families of children with disabilities. In the next, we argue that there is an urgent need to deconstruct "knowledge" about these families by problematizing the research methodologies used in the generating of this knowledge. Finally, we propose the use of narrative inquiry, theoretically grounded in sociocultural perspectives, as a critical research methodology for exploring the experiences of families of children with disabilities in a meaningful way. We argue that narrative inquiries situate the lives of these families in cultural and sociopolitical contexts, complicate conclusions drawn from medical model-based research, and contribute to new insights on the ways in which interpretations of having a child with a disability, and of disability itself, are mutually negotiated between individuals and society.

Pathologizing families of children with disabilities

The medicalization of disability

The term medicalization refers to the identification of a behavior or condition as a medical problem that requires treatment or therapeutic intervention from medical professionals (Conrad, 2004). To a large extent, conditions that become medicalized are influenced by, and in turn influence, prevailing beliefs and values in society. The medicalization of any condition becomes an effective tool for social control; as such, defining that which is different as a medical problem legitimizes certain actions that would otherwise not be considered (Conrad, 2004; Foucault, 1965). Consequently, medical and mental health professionals become agents of social control charged with protecting the "good of whole society," and curtailing the rights of those who deviate is legitimized based on "objective" science (Foucault, 1965).

The medicalization of disability had its beginnings in the rehabilitation movement. The period between 1890 and 1920 is distinctive in disability history; it was during this time that 19th century beliefs in the supernatural causes of disability gave way to medical interpretations of disability and to the identification of disability as a social and economic problem (Byrom, 2004). The legacy of the rehabilitation model forms the core of what disability studies scholars currently refer to as the medical model. The medical model lends scientific credibility to the assumption that the source of the "problems" related to disability is located within the individual body, thereby absolving society of any complicity in the lived outcomes for people with disabilities (2004). According to Linton (1998), "The medicalization of disability casts human variation as deviance from the norm, as pathological condition, as deficit, and, significantly, as an individual burden and personal tragedy" (p. 11). Furthermore, she argues that society, colluding to keep the issue of disability "within the purview of the medical establishment," reifies the idea that what needs to be "treated" is the individual with the disability, rather than "the social processes and policies that constrict disabled peoples' lives" (1998, p. 11). In medical model perspectives, disabilities are equated with a poor quality of life and are viewed as limitations to be "fixed" or overcome (Saxton, 2000). Focusing on the biological nature of disability and its impact on individual bodies, the medical model applies interpretations of disability universally. Here, the sociocultually situated nature of disability is obscured and the argument that living with a disability has as much to do with its social consequences as with its biological ones is silent.

Dominant narratives on families of children with disabilities

Despite monumental changes that have occurred during the past century with regard to the treatment of individuals with disabilities, including the deinstitutionalization movement, sweeping changes in legislation, medical advances, early intervention, and mandated public education for children with disabilities, perceptions of a poor quality of life and negative outcomes for individuals with disabilities and their families have remained relatively unchanged. Beginning in the mid-20th century, a proliferation of research literature highlighted predominantly negative outcomes for families of children with disabilities, suggesting that the experience of chronic sorrow, helplessness, guilt, psychological distress, and a persistent state of "mourning" are typical among this group (e.g., Fowle, 1968; Olshansky, 1962; Solnit & Stark, 1961). Families that institutionalized their children with disabilities were found to be more harmonious and integrated than those in which children with disabilities were raised at home (Farber, 1962), lending support to prevailing professional opinions that the institutionalization of children with disabilities was in the best interest of all concerned. In the decades that followed, researchers continued to seek to understand response patterns associated with the presence of a child with a disability in families. However, much of the literature that emerged from these efforts was situated in medical model perspectives and interpreted through a clinical lens. As a result, professionals have continued to operate on assumptions that the lives of families of children with disabilities are characterized by never-ending struggles to overcome seemingly insurmountable obstacles (Wickham-Searl, 1992). Beliefs about children with disabilities as presenting unmitigated hardship for families continue to frame dominant discourses (Ferguson, Gartner, & Lipsky, 2000) and to motivate research aimed at helping families to "cope" with their situations.

In cultural narratives about the experience of parenting a child with a disability, notions of profound loss and burden emerge as central themes, and these are upheld in institutional discourses and practices (Lalvani, 2008, 2011). Additionally, the accepted wisdom that grieving is a natural reaction to the birth of a child with a disability is often explicitly stated in literature targeted to families of children with disabilities (e.g. Trainer, 1995). Although the existence of negative psychological states and heightened levels of depression or stress among this group of families is well documented in research (e.g. Foley, 2006; Hanson & Hanline, 1990; Hendriks, De Moor, Oud, & Savelberg, 2000; Keller & Honig, 2004; Rodrigue, Morgan, & Geffken, 1990), the sociocultural conditions that construct, support, and sustain these emotions remain largely unexamined (Lalvani, 2011). The psychological distress experienced by these families is understood as originating solely from their child's disability; the experience of "grief" is decontextualized from the environments in which families exist. In this extensive body of research that seeks to explore depression and stress among these families, issues pertaining to sociocultural attitudes, values and beliefs about disability are not explored. For instance, Foley (2006) states that the birth of a child with a disability "signals a potential threat to the spirit as well as to the daily and future life of the parents" (p. 227). Here, the source of the "threat" is assumed to be the disability, which is understood as residing in the child. "Threat" is not located in sociocultural and institutional discourses that position children with disability as other; important issues pertaining to marginalization, social exclusion, and stigma are not considered. Similarly, as Ferguson (2002) points out, in Farber's (1962) study, which found families who institutionalized their children to be more harmonious, questions concerning the non-existence of supports or services for the families studied were not asked. Ironically, in studies that examine stress levels of parents of children with disabilities, the stress that these parents may encounter when dealing with professionals is typically left unexamined.

Another theme that has prevailed in dominant cultural narratives about families of children with disabilities pertains to denial. The notion that families' responses to a child's disability may be characterized by a refusal or inability to "accept" their child's condition is prevalent in clinical as well as educational discourse, and manifests itself in professional practice (Gallagher, Fialka, Rhodes, & Arceneaus, 2001). The use of the term denial in this context stems from Kubler-Ross' (1969) work on death and dying, in which she identifies the stages of grief associated with terminal illness or bereavement, with denial being one stage along the path to ultimate acceptance. Through professional training in nursing, psychology, social work or education, it is not uncommon for professionals to learn about these stages of grief in the context of families of children with disabilities, or to learn that, similar to individuals coming to terms with loss pertaining to death, these families can be expected to grieve the loss of the "perfect child" or to initially experience denial (Gallagher et al., 2001). As a result, professionals are trained to expect strongly negative reactions and a period of "mourning" among families of children with disabilities. In research literature, although there is support for the idea that these families remain in "denial" for a period of time or are unable to "adjust" to their child's disability (Butler, 1983; Luterman, 2004; Murray & Cornell, 1981), few researchers have attempted to contextualize parents' reactions, or to examine parents' interpretations of their own behaviors labeled as denial. In failing to problematize the labeling of particular responses as denial, this body of research privileges professional knowledge over parents' insights, perspectives and concerns, and lends support to notions of pathological functioning among parents of children with disabilities. On a broader level, it reifies the notion that the disability itself is "real" and absolute, and that parents' resistance to professionals' interpretations of the "problem" is in itself a problem. Traditional inquiries which theorize parents' responses to their children's disabilities as homogeneous and universal fail to acknowledge the influence of sociocultural values and beliefs about the nature and implications of having a child with a disability. Additionally, they do not evaluate the processes through which parents make sense of disability, account for variability in parents' interpretations of children's disability labels, or examine the impact of perceptions of stigma. For instance, as Green (2002) asserts, a disability diagnosis thrusts a parent into a stigmatized social category; hence, emotional distress may be the result of courtesy stigma (Goffman, 1963), or the perception of being stigmatized because of one's association with a stigmatized individual. Finally, traditional inquiries about families of children with disabilities do not even consider the possibility of positive outcomes, or perceptions of a satisfying quality of life among these families.

Disabling Research Methodologies

Although families of children with disabilities have been placed under the research microscope, the research methodologies used in providing insights into their experiences have not been subjected to the same scrutiny. Ferguson (2002) points out that just as familial adjustment to a child with a disability is embedded in cultural beliefs and values, professionals' understanding of these families are also embedded in sociocultural contexts. The interpretation of the birth of a child with a disability is a collaborative activity; the meaning of the event is negotiated between individuals and society. From this perspective, there arises a need to deconstruct prevailing knowledge about families of children with disabilities and to critically examine research methodologies used in the generating of this knowledge.

Shifting one's gaze from research findings to research methods reveals problematic conceptual frameworks rooted in the medical model and methodologies that seek to confirm pre-existing hypotheses. Traditional research, situated in beliefs that families of children with disabilities necessarily experience a quality of life that is less than satisfying sought to identify negative behavioral outcomes or patterns of dysfunction, with the goal of developing therapeutic interventions for these families. In scholarly works aimed at helping these families, research questions typically originated from researchers and were informed by theoretical frameworks that were understood as absolute and universally relevant. It should be acknowledged that although these efforts were well-intentioned, the inevitable outcome of inquiry that originated from assumptions of dysfunction was that almost all response patterns among these parents, including their attempts to resist negative assumptions regarding their experiences, were likely to be pathologized (Ferguson et al., 2000). Thus, families' displeasure over professionals' supposed lack of support could be seen as displaced anger, acceptance could be interpreted as an outgrowth of denial, and highly involved or active parents could be viewed as demonstrating overcompensating behavior (2000). Additionally, this body of research failed to capture nuances in familial reactions, as well as parents' perceptions of positive outcomes or personal transformations resulting from their unique parenting experiences.

In studies that adhered to a positivist myth of neutrality in scientific research, the use of attitudinal scales and other instruments thought to "objectively" measure families' response patterns and perceptions was not uncommon. Here, the instruments used such as the Parenting Stress Index (PSI), the Parenting Stress Scale (PSS), and the Global Inventory of Stress (GIS) are worthy of mention, because, as their names suggest, they are indicative that assumptions of inordinate levels of stress among families of children with disabilities were the starting points for inquiry in these studies. Additionally, as Schwarz (1999) argues, self-report measures of this nature are potentially problematic, because the wording of the questions and the format are likely to shape participants' responses. For instance, in a study that investigated the limitations of self-report questionnaires, when reporting life events and satisfaction, participants who were asked to report positive events reported higher levels of happiness and life satisfaction, while participants asked to report negative events reported lower levels of happiness and life satisfaction; the nature of the questions made particular emotions more accessible and salient (Strack, Schwarz, & Gschneidinger, 1985). As Schwarz (1999) explains:

As researchers, we tend to view our questionnaires as "measurement devices" that elicit information from respondents. What we frequently overlook is that our questionnaires are also a source of information that respondents draw on…[researchers] miss the extent to which the questions we ask determine the answers we receive. (p. 103)

This raises concerns about the potential for parenting stress inventories to elicit negative responses, due to questions that focus on stress, burden, and negative emotions. Furthermore, although these instruments present a range of questions within the child and parent domains, questions pertaining to parents' understanding of societal, cultural, or institutional discourses and practices are not typically included. In many instruments that seek to determine parents' perceived levels of satisfaction or efficacy, there is little acknowledgement that these perceptions exist in the context of an environment that shapes and upholds certain realities and silences others. Perceptions of parenthood are examined as if they exist in a vacuum or in isolation from social and cultural contexts.

In traditional research which begins with assumptions of negative outcomes, questions originate from researchers' perspectives rather than from those of the families, and as such, in this body of literature, the interpretations of the family members are largely missing. It should also be acknowledged that when research is conducted in the context of a therapeutic relationship or under a clinical lens, it assumes a stance of profound unequalness and privileges certain interpretations over others. Although, it can certainly be argued that almost all research is situated in power differentials between the observer and the observed, in the case where research participants are in a therapeutic relationship with researchers, or the aim is to generate therapeutic interventions, strong assumptions about the quality of life and psychological functioning among those being studied are being made. Furthermore, when the focus tends to be on the behavior and mental health of individuals, problems inherent in societal and institutional structures and practices are of less concern, and critical questions pertaining to societal barriers to optimal family functioning, institutional and cultural ableism, or problems inherent in society are left unexplored. Ultimately, in research that locates the experiences of families of children with disabilities as outside the normative range, these families become positioned as other, and their members are denied the right to define their lives in their own terms.

The limitations of traditional positivist methodologies rooted in the medical model call for a theoretical shift in the overall concept of disability, as well as the methods of inquiry used to study the experiences of families of children with disabilities. To address the inherent limitations of medical model-based studies, we discuss three critical theoretical frameworks that have the potential to inform, extend, and contextualize existing knowledge about families of children with disabilities: critical disability theory, sociocultural theory, and positioning theory.

Critical Theoretical Frameworks: Contextualizing Families of Children with Disabilities

Critical disability theory

Traditional theory conceptualizes the researcher as objective and disinterested from the object of study, and holds that the goal of research is to "describe the world as it is" (Hosking, 2008). In contrast, rather than observing and explaining a fixed and objective reality, critical theory focuses on power and privilege, seeking to emancipate and challenge oppression and domination (Hosking, 2008; Mertens, 2007). Grounded in the stance that multiple realities exist, critical theory holds that it is imperative to include the voices of historically disenfranchised groups in research, and for knowledge to emerge through social interactions between researchers and participants (Mertens, 2007).

Based on the stance that individuals with disabilities have been denied full access to mainstream life and have been subjected to the same forms of discrimination and segregation as members of other oppressed groups (Hahn, 1997; Saxton, 2000), critical disability theory contends that concepts of normality and disability are strongly influenced by those in positions of power and control, such as members of the medical establishment (Kliewer, 1998). Situated in social model perspectives, critical disability theory conceptualizes disability as a sociocultural construct as opposed to a biological reality. In contrast with inquiries based in the medical model, research based in critical paradigms understand the experience of disability as an interrelationship between impairment and interpretations of impairment, and focus on the attitudinal and institutional barriers faced by individuals with disabilities (Horkheimer, 1972; Hahn, 1997). Through this lens, the lived experience of disability is inextricably linked with the sociocultural meaning of disability, and with the language and practices which uphold the privilege of nondisabled people (Bogdan & Taylor, 1994; Danforth & Navarro, 1998; Finlay, Lyons, & Taylor, 2005; Hosking, 2008; Taylor, 2000). Focusing its gaze on imbalances in power and systematic oppression, critical disability theory is "intentionally political" and transformative in nature (Hosking, 2008, p. 17).

Sociocultural theory

Rooted in the work of Lev Vygotsky, sociocultural theory stresses the interpersonal nature of psychological phenomena, positing that human thought and development emerge through social interaction (Vygotsky, 1978). At the crux of Vygotskian sociocultural psychology is the idea that individual mental processes can only be understood by examining the social and cultural processes from which they derive (Wertsch & Tulviste, 1994). From this perspective, the experience of disability, and of families of children with disabilities, are understood as situated in the beliefs, values, and attitudes that exist in a given culture at a particular time in history. Sociocultural theory provides a lens for exploring the situated nature and meaning of disability. The medical model perspective, which conceptualizes disability as necessarily associated with decreased quality of life, or as biologically determined (Asch, 1989; Saxton, 2000), therefore reflects "a moment in a cultural focus" (McDermott & Varenne, 1995, p. 324), as opposed to an objective reality. Alternatively, framed in sociocultural theory, the phenomenon of disability is understood as socially constructed, and individual parents' adaptation to the birth of a child with a disability is best understood in the context of the ways in which this experience is culturally interpreted or the meaning that parents attach to this life event.

Positioning theory

Positioning theory offers an explanation of the ways in which psychological and social phenomena are constructed and sustained through discourse and activity in which individuals position themselves and others (Harre & Gillet, 1994). Here, the concept of fluid "positioning" replaces the notion of static "roles" that individuals possess in social relationships (1994). Through social interaction, claims are made about social relationships, power dynamics are enacted, and particular interpretations of the world are upheld, with some individuals benefiting more than others (Harre & Moghaddam, 2003; Harre & Slocum, 2003). Medical model perspectives, in which families of children with disabilities are "malignantly positioned" (Parrott, 2003) as unhappy and maladjusted reflect this imposition of identities and demonstrate the ways in which power dynamics shape a constructed reality.

Positioning theory uses the lens of master narratives and counter narratives. Master narratives are understood as "frames according to which courses of events can be easily plotted, simply because one's audience is taken to 'know' and accept these courses" (Bamberg, 2004, p. 360). Notions about the lives of families of children with disabilities as characterized by unmitigated hardship, burden, and grief can be understood as problematic master narratives. Simultaneously, individuals can oppose dominant constructions of their experiences through their critical "counter-narratives" (Solis, 2004). As individuals construct counter-narratives, they fashion accounts that challenge power relationships and social expectations, hence opening a space to problematize particular viewpoints on human experiences.

Narrative Inquiries: Findings from Critical Research

A growing body of research situated in critical theoretical frameworks and using narratives as a methodological tool suggests that families do not experience the presence of a child with a disability in a homogeneous or universally negative manner and that there is a wide range of outcomes for these families (Ferguson et al., 2000). Indeed, after an initial period of uncertainty, many families of children with disabilities may be more accurately characterized by resilience or thriving than by burden and grief, and overall, their perceptions of their quality of life seem to resemble those of families in general (Ferguson, 2002; Hastings & Taunt, 2002; Van Riper, 2007). Beyond issues of adjustment and resilience, research that elicits family narratives attends to the extent to which interpretations of this experience are situated in the sociocultural meanings ascribed to the constructs of disability, normalcy, and parenthood, as well as in institutional discourses and practices that position children with disabilities and their families as other (Goddard, Lehr, & Lapadat, 2000; Lalvani, 2011).

Similar to Mishler's (1995) style of interviewing which allows researchers and participants to co-construct the meaning of the experiences and events that are reported, Goddard et al. (2000) engaged parents of children with disabilities in critically constructing the subjective meaning they attached to their experiences. These parents had agency in identifying "problems" as well as in interpreting the nature of these. Additionally they were asked to prioritize the concerns they had raised, thereby increasing our understanding of the salience of the various issues among them. The findings indicated that for many parents in this study, perceptions of stereotyping by professionals were a source of concern. Their narratives also shed light on "stories of difference" in which parents discussed the subjugated gaze of others and articulated perceptions that they were made to feel guilty through a hyper-scrutiny of their parenting styles or of the prenatal choices they had made, with its implicit message that these parents might somehow have caused their children's disabilities. This finding compels us to rethink master narratives on "guilt" as a natural reaction among parents of children with disabilities, suggesting instead that rigid social structures which implicitly or explicitly portray parents as the cause of the problem may construct feelings of guilt among some parents. Furthermore, in this study parents discussed their frustrations with bureaucracy involved in obtaining services and with professionals' pathologizing of their children's behaviors.

Narrative inquiries reveal that the experience of parenting a child with a disability is linked with issues of stigma. Some studies shed light on master narratives in which families of children with disabilities are positioned as non-normative, and indicated that parents' emotional distress was linked to their perceptions of being stigmatized and to their beliefs that their children with disabilities were less "valued" in society (Birenbaum, 1970; Goddard et al., 2000; Green, 2003; Lalvani, 2011; Landsman, 1999). Lalvani (2011) explored this in depth through open-ended questions which allowed mothers to reflect on their experiences of the birth of their children with Down syndrome. In this study mothers believed that through their interactions with medical and mental health professionals they had received negative interpretations of Down syndrome. Some recalled that doctors had used the language of "tragedy" and "burden" in conveying their children's diagnoses, and some others believed they had experienced subtle or overt coercion to consider terminating their pregnancies after a prenatal diagnosis of Down syndrome. Professionals were not the only sources of negative messages for mothers in this study; many related accounts of friends or family members who had reacted to the news of their children's having Down syndrome with profound sadness or expressions of sympathy. Additionally, some recalled that it was not uncommon for people to ask them whether they had undergone prenatal genetic testing or to express puzzlement over why they had chosen to forgo it, with the implicit message that having a child with Down syndrome is a largely undesirable experience and one that these mothers could have prevented. This is resonant of the argument that interpretations of the birth of a child with a disability and the very meaning of motherhood may be profoundly shaped by the technologies of prenatal genetic testing and by an increasing cultural expectations of birthing a "perfect child" (Parens & Asch, 2003; Rapp, 2000; Rothman, 1993). Lalvani's (2011) study highlighted numerous ways in which families of children with disabilities become positioned in hegemonic discourse as other. For instance, mothers recalled that in addition to being recipients of sympathy, they were also recipients of admiration; i.e. people would comment that they were "brave," "courageous," "blessed," or "chosen by god" to have "a special baby." Many mothers believed that although these comments were well-intentioned, they also conveyed negative messages about their children as "not normal" or "not perfect" and therefore not desirable. These findings shed light on the ways in which parents' interpretations of their children's disabilities are situated in constructions of normative families and valued children.

Narrative inquiries draw critical attention to the ways in which parents' responses to their children's disabilities are situated in cultural interpretations of disability labels and of the parameters of "normalcy" (Harry, 1992, 1997; Harry, Allen, & McLaughlin, 1995). Through extensive ethnographic data collected from a group of Puerto Rican parents, Harry (1992) found that these parents reacted strongly to labels of "mental retardation" ascribed to their children because of cultural association of this term with mental illness and its translation to the term "loco" or "crazy." Additionally among these parents, parameters of normalcy were broader than would be expected in a Western psychological context. For instance, one mother in this study expressed incredulousness that her six and a half year old daughter, who was bilingual and able to read, could be described by professionals as "mentally retarded." Additionally in this study, parents' interpretations of their children's disability labels were embedded in their understandings of personal and family identity; the cultural associations between mental retardation and mental illness took on greater significance for parents because of their perceptions that this reflected on their entire family. This is similar to Lalvani's (2012) study in which many parents offered their own theories on why they dispute specific labels for their children. Some explained that they believed labels would lower expectations or stigmatize their children and others, like parents in Harry's study, had very different interpretations of "impaired" cognitive functioning. The information gained through these two studies is critical to understanding professional-parent partnerships in schools. In the educational arena, Harry (1992) points out that parents whose perspectives differ from those held by professionals or those who contest the specific labels ascribed to their children are often viewed as being resistant or unwilling to accept their children's conditions. Using medical-model frameworks, these parents' behaviors are often attributed to their being in "denial." However, their narratives elucidate that the categorizations of these parents as being "in denial" are unjustified. As clearly seen in the two studies discussed above, parents who resisted particular disability labels did not dispute that their children had some learning difficulties or delays in development; rather, their resistance pertained largely to the naming of the "problem" (Harry, 1992; Lalvani, 2012). This is consistent with the assertions of scholars who deconstruct notions of denial among families of children with disabilities and who argue that behaviors attributed to "being in denial" may in fact be interpreted by some parents as maintaining hope, remaining optimistic about their children's future, or perceiving their children as worthwhile (Gallagher et al., 2001; Hartshorne, 2002).

The capacity for human agency is exemplified in research that privileges the voices of families of children with disabilities. Consistent with the idea that when positioned in master narratives, individuals are agentic in producing counter-narratives that assign new meaning to interpersonal and psychic phenomena (Bamberg, 2004), the findings of critical research on families of children with disabilities capture contested perspectives on their experiences. In these inquiries, families resisted dominant discourses of otherness, rejected notions of sorrow and grief, and repositioned themselves as "normal" families (Fisher & Goodley, 2007; Goddard et al., 2000; Green, 2003, Lalvani, 2011; Taylor, 2000). As an example, in Fisher and Godley's (2007) study in which parents were empowered to theorize and offer analyses of their own stories, parents articulated their resistance to stigma and to the strong cultural emphasis on normalcy. Furthermore, rejecting the notion that negative experiences dominate their lives, they chose to focus their narratives on their own positive transformations and on the ways in which they have been able to enjoy their children "as they are." Similarly, in a number of other studies, parents of children with disabilities discuss both the stressors and the positive aspects of their parenting experiences and highlight perceptions of their own personal growth (Scallan, Senior, & Reilly, 2010; Scorgie & Sobsey, 2000; Scorgie, Wilgosh, Sobsey, & McDonald, 2001; Skinner, Bailey, Correa, & Rodriguez, 1999). Additionally, many parents in these studies understood their stressors as aspects of parenting, rather than parenting a child with a disability. In a different way, Taylor's (2000) ethnographic account of the Duke family, a low-income family in which each member had a disability label, sheds light on the ways in which this family framed its identity in ways that resisted notions of damage and stigma. Although each member of this family was bombarded with labels through health care and welfare agencies as well as schools, its members were able to maintain positive identity as a family by thinking of individual members in ways that were removed from their labels. For instance, family members receiving special education services or psychiatric treatment were described as "acting young for his age," "being a pest," "drinks too much," or "immature" rather than being "mentally retarded" or "mentally ill." Thus they maintained a positive social identity because they defined the parameters of normalcy quite broadly.

In parents' counter-narratives, concerns related to the discrimination, rejection, or social isolation of children with disabilities' experiences emerge as an important theme (Green, 2002, 2003; Lalvani, 2008), which further illustrates that these families locate disability not solely in their child, but also in the sociocultural environment. This is evident in Green's (2002) narrative account of raising her daughter with cerebral palsy, in which she states: "I envisioned a future of personal rejection, torturous teasing, and social isolation for my daughter. She would have no friends, no love in her life, no family in the future" (p. 31). However, though she identifies the sadness or "despair" which has characterized her life at times, she also states:

…the experience of mothering Amanda has changed dramatically, irrevocably, and in very positive ways who I am and the ways in which I view the world… It has also broadened my image of what it means to live a good and worthwhile life… (Green, 2002, p. 30)

Unlike the findings of positivist research, Green's narrative provides a rich and nuanced understanding of her mothering experiences and informs us that although negative emotions may be aspects of parenting a child with a disability, these are situated in the context of a society that isolates, marginalizes, or does not fully accept individuals with disabilities. Furthermore, it informs us that parenting a child with a disability is a complex, multi-layered experience in which, in addition to difficulties, parents also experience great joy, derive new meaning, and often find themselves transformed. Overall, the findings from narrative inquiries complicate core assumptions about disability as self-contained impairment and reveal transformations in parents' own interpretations of the familial experience of having a child with a disability. Such critical work can be instrumental in contributing to a broader dialogue on the fluid and contextualized meanings of disability and cultural definitions of normative family life.

Conclusion

Narrative inquiries rooted in the central tenets of critical disability theory, sociocultural theory, and positioning theory facilitate an epistemological shift in the manner in which families of children with disabilities are studied and understood. These critical inquiries situate families in context and allow for opportunities to better understand how responses to disabilities emerge within complex social dynamics at particular cultural and historical "moments." Families of children with disabilities engage in meaning making as they negotiate dominant medical discourses on disability; socioculturally-based research opens a space for families to complicate and contest this construction of their experiences. These families counter-narrate their experiences and challenge and complicate medical assumptions regarding the meaning of having a child with a disability. Critical narrative inquiries contribute to the emerging discourse on the sociocultural meaning of disability. Through the lens of sociocultural approaches, disability is not a self-contained or universally defined experience; instead, it is one that is relational and constructed. Ultimately, sociocultural inquiries allow researchers to critically evaluate not only the meaning of disability, but also the parameters of normalcy that permeate our construction of childhood, and more specifically, of the desired child.

References

  • Asch, A. (1989). Reproductive technology and disability. In S. Cohen & N. Taub (Eds.), Reproductive laws for the 1990's (pp. 69-124). Clifton, NJ: Humana Press.
  • Bamberg, M. (2004). Considering counter narratives. In M. Bamberg & M. Andrews (Eds.), Considering counter narratives (pp. 351-371). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
  • Birenbaum, A. (1970). On managing a courtesy stigma. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 11, 196-206.
  • Bogdan, R., & Taylor, S. J. (1994). The social meaning of mental retardation: Two life stories. New York: Teachers College Press.
  • Byrom, B. (2004). A pupil and a patient. In S. Danforth & S. Taff (Eds.), Crucial readings in special education (pp. 25-37). New Jersey: Pearson.
  • Conrad, P. (2004). The discovery of hyperkinesis: Notes on the medicalization of deviant behavior. In S. Danforth & S. Taff (Eds.), Crucial readings in special education (pp. 18-24). New Jersey: Pearson.
  • Danforth, S., & Navarro, V. (1998). Speech acts: Sampling the social construction of mental retardation in everyday life. Mental Retardation 36(1), 31-43.
  • Farber, B. (1962). Effects of a severely mentally retarded child on the family. In E. P. Trapp & P. Himelstein (Eds.), Readings on the exceptional child: Research and theory (pp. 227-246). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Ferguson, P. H. (2002). A place in the family: An historical interpretation of research on parental reactions to having a child with a disability. Journal of Special Education, 36(3), 124-130.
  • Ferguson, P. M., Gartner, A., & Lipsky, D. K. (2000). The experience of disability in families: A synthesis of research and parent narratives. In E. Parens & A. Asch (Eds.), Prenatal testing and disability rights (pp. 72-94). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
  • Finlay, W. M. L., Lyons, E., & Taylor, S. J. (2005). Rejecting the label: A social constructionist analysis. Mental Retardation, 43(2), 120-234.
  • Fisher, P., & Goodley, D. (2007). The linear medical model of disability: Mothers of disabled babies resist with counter-narratives. Sociology of Health and Illness, 29(1), 66-81.
  • Foley, G. M. (2006). The loss-grief cycle: Coming to terms with the birth of a child with a disability. In G. Foley and J. Hochman (Eds.), Mental health in early intervention: Achieving unity in principles and practice (pp. 227-243). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brooks.
  • Foucault, M. (1965). Madness and civilization. New York: Pantheon.
  • Fowle, C. (1968). The effect of the severely mentally retarded child on his family. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 73, 468-473.
  • Gallagher, P. A., Fialka, J., Rhodes, C., & Arceneaus, C. (2001). Working with families: Rethinking denial. Young Exceptional Children, (5)2.
  • Goddard, J., Lehr, R., & Lapadat, J. (2000). Parents of children with disabilities: Telling a different story. Canadian Journal of Counseling, 34(4), 273-289.
  • Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. New York: Simon & Schuster.
  • Green, S. E. (2002). Mothering Amanda: Musing on the experience of raising a child with cerebral palsy. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 7, 21-34.
  • Green, S. E. (2003). What do you mean 'what's wrong with her?': Stigma and the lives of families of children with disabilities. Social Science & Medicine, 57(8), 1361-1374.
  • Hahn, H. (1997). Advertising the acceptably employable image. In L. Davis (Ed.), The disability studies reader (pp. 172-186). New York: Routledge.
  • Hanson, M. J., & Hanline, M. F. (1990). Parenting a child with a disability: A longitudinal study of parental stress and adaptation. Journal of Early Intervention, 14, 234-248.
  • Harre, R., & Gillet, G. (1994). The discursive mind. Los Angeles: Sage.
  • Harre, R., & Moghaddam, F. (2003). Introduction: The self and others in traditional psychology and in positioning theory. In R. Harre & F. Moghaddam (Eds.), The self and others: Positioning individuals and groups in personal, political, and cultural contexts. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.
  • Harre, R., & Slocum, N. (2003). Disputes as complex social events: On the uses of positioning theory. In R. Harre & F. Moghaddam (Eds.), The self and others: Positioning individuals and groups in personal, political, and cultural contexts. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.
  • Harry, B. (1992). Making sense of disability: Low-income, Puerto-Rican parents' theories of the problem. Exceptional Children, 59, 27-40.
  • Harry, B., Allen, N., & McLaughlin, M. (1995). Communication versus compliance: African-American parents' involvement in special education. Exceptional Children, 61(4), 364-377.
  • Hartshorne, T. S. (2002). Mistaking courage for denial: Family resilience after the birth of a child with severe disabilities. Journal of Individual Psychology, 58(3), 263-278.
  • Hastings, R. P., & Taunt, H. M. (2002). Positive perceptions in families of children with developmental disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 107(2), 116-127.
  • Hendriks, A. H. C., De Moor, J. M. H., Oud, J. H. L., & Savelberg, M. H. W. (2000). Perceived changes in well-being of parents with a child in a therapeutic toddler class. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 21, 455-468.
  • Horkheimer, M. (1972). Critical theory: Selected essays. New York: The Continuum Publishing Company.
  • Hosking, D. L. (2008). Critical disability theory. 4th Biennial Disability Studies Conference. Lancaster University, UK.
  • Keller, D., & Honig, A. S. (2004). Maternal and paternal stress in families with school-aged children with disabilities. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 74(3), 337-348.
  • Kliewer, C. (1998). Schooling children with Down syndrome: Toward an understanding of possibility. New York: Teachers College Press.
  • Kubler-Ross, E. (1969). On death and dying. New York: Macmillan.
  • Lalvani, P. (2008). Mothers of children with Down syndrome: Constructing the sociocultural meaning of disability. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 46(6), 436-445.
  • Lalvani, P. (2011). Constructing the (m)other. Dominant and contested narratives on mothering a child with Down syndrome. Narrative Inquiry, 21(2). 276-293.
  • Lalvani, P. (2012). Parents' participation in special education in the context of implicit educational ideologies and socioeconomic privilege. Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities.
  • Landsman, G. (1999). Does God give special kids to special parents? Personhood and the child with disabilities as gift and giver. In L Lane (Ed.), Transformative motherhood: On giving and getting in a consumer culture (pp. 133-165). New York: NYU Press.
  • Linton, S. (1998). Claiming disability: Knowledge and identity. New York: NYU Press.
  • Luterman, D. (2004). Counseling families of children with hearing loss and special needs. The Volta Review, 104(4) 215-220.
  • McDermott, R., & Varenne, H. (1995). Culture "as" disability. Anthropology & Education Quarterly, 26(3), 324-348.
  • Mertens, D. M. (2007). Transformative paradigm: Mixed methods and social justice. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 1(3), 212-225.
  • Mishler, E. (1995). Models of narrative analysis: A typology. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 5(2), 87-123.
  • Murray, J. N., & Cornell, C. J. (1981). Parentalplegia. Psychology in the Schools, 18, 201-207.
  • Olshansky, S. (1962). Chronic sorrow: A response to having a mentally defective child. Social Casework, 43, 190-193.
  • Parens, E., & Asch, A. (2003). Disability rights critique of prenatal genetic testing: Reflections and recommendations. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 9, 40-47.
  • Parrott, W. G. (2003). Motivational styles and positioning theory. In R. Harre & F. Moghaddam (Eds.), The self and others: Positioning individuals and groups in personal, political, and cultural contexts. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.
  • Rapp, R. (2000). Testing women, testing the fetus. New York: Routledge.
  • Rodrigue, J. R., Morgan, S. B., & Geffken, G. (1990). Families of autistic children: Psychological functioning of mothers. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 19, 371-379.
  • Rothman, B. K. (1993). The tentative pregnancy: How amniocentesis changes the experience of motherhood. New York: W. W. Norton & Co. Ltd.
  • Saxton, M. (2000). Why members of the disability community oppose prenatal diagnosis and selective abortion. In E. Parens & A. Asch (Eds.), Prenatal testing and disability rights (pp. 147-164). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
  • Scallan, S., Senior, J., & Reilly, C. (2010). Williams syndrome: Daily challenges and positive impact on the family. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 24(2), 181-188.
  • Schwarz, N. (1999). Self-reports: How the questions shape the answers. American Psychologist, 54(2), 93-105.
  • Scorgie, K., & Sobsey, D. (2000). Transformational outcomes associated with parenting children who have disabilities. Mental Retardation, 38(3), 195-206.
  • Scorgie, K., Wilgosh, L., Sobsey, D., & McDonald, J. (2001). Parent life management and transformational outcomes when a child has Down syndrome. International Journal of Special Education, 16(2), 57-68.
  • Skinner, D., Bailey, D. B. Jr., Correa, V., & Rodriguez, P. (1999). Narrating self and disability: Latino mothers' construction of identities vis-a-vis their child with special needs. Exceptional Children, 65(4), 81-95.
  • Solis, J. (2004). Narrating and counter-narrating illegality as an identity. In C. Daiute & C. Lightfoot (Eds.), Narrative analysis: Studying the development of individuals in society. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
  • Solnit, A., & Stark, M. (1961). Mourning and the birth of a defective child. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 16, 523-537.
  • Strack, F., Schwarz, N., & Gschneidinger, E. (1985). Happiness and reminiscing: The role of time perspective, mood, and mode of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1460-1469.
  • Taylor, S. J. (2000). You're not a retard, you're just wise: Disability, social identity and family networks. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 29, 58-92.
  • Trainer, M. (1995). Adjusting to your new baby. In K. Stray-Gunderson (Ed.), Babies with Down syndrome: A new parents' guide (pp. 37-62). Bethesda, MD: Woodbine House.
  • Van Riper, M. (2007). Families of children with Down syndrome: Responding to a change of plans with resilience. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 22(2), 116-128.
  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Wertsch, J. V., & Tulviste, P. (1994). Lev Semyenovich Vygotsky and contemporary developmental psychology. In R. Parke, P. Ornstein, J. Rieser, & C. Zahn-Waxler (Eds.), A century of developmental psychology (pp. 333- 356). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  • Wickham-Searl, P. (1992). Mother with a mission. In P. M. Ferguson & S. J. Taylor (Eds.), Interpreting disability: A qualitative reader (pp. 251-274). New York: Teachers College Press.
Return to Top of Page